Introduction
As illustrated by the present Research Topic, emotion that can either enhance or hinder
various aspects of our cognition and behavior. For instance, the emotional charge
of an event can increase attention to and memory for that event (Dolcos et al., 2012),
whereas task-irrelevant emotional information may lead to increased distraction away
from goal-relevant tasks (Iordan et al., 2013; see also Dolcos et al., 2011). Interestingly,
sometimes these opposing effects of emotion co-occur. For example, hearing a gunshot
may enhance memory for central aspects of what was happening at the time, while impairing
memory for peripheral details (Christianson, 1992). It is also possible that increased
distraction from ongoing goals produced by task-irrelevant emotional stimuli may lead
to better memory for the distracting information itself. The co-occurrence of enhancing
and impairing effects of emotion is probably most evident in affective disorders,
where both of these opposing effects are exacerbated. Specifically, uncontrolled recollection
of and rumination on distressing memories observed in depression and post-traumatic
stress disorder (PTSD) may also lead to impaired cognition due to enhanced emotional
distraction. Here, we illustrate an example based on evidence from studies of PTSD,
pointing to the importance of investigating both enhancing and impairing effects of
emotion, in elucidating the nature of alterations in the way emotion interacts with
cognition in clinical conditions.
Background: emotional and cognitive processing in PTSD
Changes in emotional and cognitive processing are critical features in PTSD patients,
typically reflected in increased emotional reactivity and recollection of traumatic
memories, along with impaired cognitive/executive control (Rauch et al., 2006; Shin
and Liberzon, 2009; see also in this issue Brown and Morey, 2012; Hayes et al., 2012).
Of particular note is emerging evidence concerning the neural correlates of alterations
associated with the encoding of emotional memories (Hayes et al., 2011) and with the
responses to task-irrelevant emotional distraction (Morey et al., 2009). These changes
are reflected in regions associated with functions that may be enhanced (episodic
memory) or impaired (working memory) by emotion—i.e., the medial temporal lobe (MTL)
and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), respectively. Here, we illustrate how
understanding the changes associated with the way traumatic memories are formed and
retrieved in PTSD (involving MTL areas) may clarify their impact on ongoing cognitive/executive
processes (reflected in changes of dlPFC activity), when potential cues for traumatic
memories are presented as task irrelevant distracters.
The enhancing effect of emotion
Studies investigating the memory-enhancing effect of emotion in healthy participants
point to the role of basic MTL mechanisms involving interactions between emotion-based
regions (amygdala—AMY) and memory-related regions (hippocampus and associated parahippocampal
cortices—HC, PHC) in the formation and retrieval of emotional memories (Dolcos et
al., 2012). Neurobiological models of PTSD (Layton and Krikorian, 2002) propose that
the development and maintenance of the disorder is linked to altered activity in the
MTL during encoding of traumatic memories. Hence, intrusive recollection of traumatic
memories observed in PTSD may be linked to dysfunction of the basic MTL mechanism
identified in healthy participants as being responsible for the memory-enhancing effect
of emotion (Dolcos et al., 2004). Specifically, processing of cues related to traumatic
events may trigger recollection of traumatic memories, which due to dysfunctional
interactions between AMY and the MTL memory system may engage a self-sustaining functional
loop in which emotion processing in AMY may enhance recollection by increasing activity
in HC; this, in turn, may intensify AMY activity as a result of re-experiencing the
emotions associated with the recollected memories (Dolcos et al., 2005; McNally, 2006).
On the other hand, there is also evidence suggesting a disconnect between the effects
observed in AMY and their link to emotional or cognitive aspects of processing in
PTSD patients. Specifically, while greater AMY activation is identified in studies
of symptom provocation (Rauch et al., 2000; Hendler et al., 2003; Shin et al., 2004,
2005; Williams et al., 2006), such an effect is not observed in studies of cognitive
processing (Shin et al., 2001; Clark et al., 2003; Bremner et al., 2004; Morey et
al., 2008).
An important observation that has emerged in the PTSD literature may reconcile this
apparent discrepancy. Specifically, there is evidence that memories for negative events
in PTSD patients may be non-specific, gist-based, rather than detailed, context-based
(McNally et al., 1994; Kaspi et al., 1995; Harvey et al., 1998). Gist refers to familiarity-based
retrieval of memories for the general meaning of a situation or event, rather than
recollection of specific contextual details (Tulving, 1985). Given that gist-based
memories are often inaccurate (Roediger and McDermott, 1995; Wright and Loftus, 1998)
and susceptible to enhanced rate of false alarms that may diminish or cancel an actual
enhancing impact of emotion on memory (Dolcos et al., 2005), it may be the case that
the basic AMY-MTL mechanisms typically responsible for the memory-enhancing effect
of emotion are in fact attenuated in PTSD. Hence, this could explain the non-specific,
gist-based, memories observed in these patients. This idea is supported by recent
findings from a fMRI study using the subsequent memory paradigm with emotional stimuli
in PTSD patients (Hayes et al., 2011), which showed reduced memory-related activity
in the AMY-MTL system during memory encoding, and higher false alarm rates during
retrieval, compared to a trauma exposed control (TEC) participants (Figure 1A). Moreover,
the PTSD patients also lacked the anterior posterior dissociation along the longitudinal
axis of the MTL, with respect to its involvement during successful encoding of emotional
memories, which was initially identified in healthy participants (Dolcos et al., 2004),
but such dissociation was preserved in the TEC group (Hayes et al., 2011). Together,
these findings suggest a disorganization of the MTL mechanisms involved in the memory-enhancing
effect of emotion in PTSD, which leads to inefficient encoding of information for
trauma-related stimuli and subsequent non-specific gist-based retrieval.
Figure 1
(A–B) Memory-Related Changes in the Medial Temporal Lobe Activity in PTSD. Reduced
memory-related activity (Dm) in AMY for trauma-related pictures, in the PTSD group
(A); a similar effect was also observed in the HC (not shown). Reduced Dm for trauma-related
pictures in the anterior HC linked to increased symptoms of arousal (B). Dm, Difference
due to Memory effect (brain activity for Remembered minus Forgotten items); PTSD,
post-traumatic stress disorder; TEC, Trauma-Exposed Control group; AMY, Amygdala;
HC, Hippocampus; PHC, Parahippocampal Cortex; Ant, Anterior; Post, Posterior. Error
bars represent the standard error of means. Adapted from Hayes et al. (2011), with
permission. (C) Evidence for Non-specific Response in dlPFC to Trauma-Related and
Neutral Distracters in PTSD. Comparison of mean percentage signal change in dlPFC
during the active maintenance period of a working memory task in the PTSD and Trauma-Exposed
Control (TEC) groups point to a generalized dlPFC disruption of activation for salient
task-irrelevant distracter scenes in the PTSD group, which showed an undifferentiated
response in the dlPFC to combat and neutral distracters. The TEC group showed disruption
in the same area, but specific to combat-related distraction. dlPFC, dorsolateral
prefrontal cortex. Adapted from Morey et al. (2009), with permission. (D) Diagram
illustrating a possible link between the impact of emotion on long-term memory and
working memory in PTSD.
The impairing effect of emotion
Studies investigating the neural correlates of the impairing effect of task-irrelevant
emotional distraction on cognitive performance identified distinct patterns of responses
in emotion and cognitive control brain regions (i.e., increased activity in AMY and
reduced activity in dlPFC, respectively), which are specific to emotional distraction
(Dolcos et al., 2011). On the one hand, based on this evidence, increased emotional
reactivity linked to changes in the AMY function in PTSD may lead to increased specific
disruption of dlPFC activity by emotional distraction. On the other hand, there is
evidence for a non-specific heightened sensitivity to both threatening and non-threatening
stimuli in PTSD (Grillon and Morgan, 1999; Peri et al., 2000), which may explain increased
distractibility to trauma related and unrelated stimuli alike.
The fact that information unrelated to the trauma may also be highly distracting in
PTSD patients is consistent with the clinically observed symptom of hypervigilance
in these patients (American Psychiatric Association, 2000), and with the evidence
for non-specific encoding of trauma-related material discussed above (Hayes et al.,
2011). Specifically, it is reasonable to expect that seemingly neutral stimuli that
may remind of trauma could act as cues for non-specific retrieval of trauma-related
information, which in turn may be as distracting as the trauma-related stimuli themselves.
Evidence from a recent study of WM with trauma-related and non-related distraction
is consistent with this idea (Morey et al., 2009). Using an adaptation of our WM task
with emotional distraction (Dolcos and McCarthy, 2006), the study by Morey and colleagues
investigated how trauma-related task-irrelevant emotional information modulates WM
networks in PTSD. Similar to the study on memory encoding discussed above, recent
post-9/11 war veterans were divided into a PTSD group and a TEC group. Functional
MRI results showed that the PTSD group had greater trauma-specific activation than
the control group in main emotion processing brain regions, including the AMY and
ventrolateral PFC (vlPFC), as well as in brain regions susceptible to emotion modulation
(e.g., fusiform gyrus—FG). However, the PTSD group also showed greater non-specific
disruption of activity to combat-related and neutral task-irrelevant distracters in
brain regions that subserve the ability to maintain focus on goal-relevant information,
including the dlPFC. This suggests a more generalized dlPFC disruption in the PTSD
group than in the control group, which showed disruption specific to the trauma-related
distraction. The undifferentiated dlPFC response to combat and non-combat distracters
in PTSD is consistent with the hypervigilance hypothesis that may explain enhanced
response to and distracting effect of neutral stimuli (Figure 1C). This neural-level
finding was complemented by the behavioral results, which showed lower overall working
memory performance for task-irrelevant distracters scenes in the PTSD group, in the
absence of a differential impact between combat-related and neutral distracters.
The link between enhancing and impairing effects of emotion
Overall, the evidence from separate lines of investigations discussed above, regarding
the neural changes in PTSD linked to dysfunctions in the recollection of traumatic
events and the response to emotional distraction, converge toward the idea that non-specific
response to emotional and neutral distraction may reflect retrieval distortions linked
to inefficient initial encoding of trauma-related information. Namely, it is possible
that the non-specific disruption of the dlPFC activity by trauma-related and neutral
distraction is linked to the retrieval of the traumatic memories triggered by non-specific
cues, which may also contribute to the perpetuation of the state of hyperarousal observed
in these patients (Figure 1D). Moreover, it is also possible that the source of these
effects may be linked to elevated arousal during the initial exposure to traumatic
events. Consistent with this idea, in addition to showing non-specific activity to
subsequently remembered items in AMY and MTL memory system in PTSD, the study by Hayes
and colleagues discussed above (Hayes et al., 2011) also identified a negative co-variation
of memory-related hippocampal activity for trauma-related items with scores of hyperarousal
symptoms, as measured with the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale (Figure 1B). In other
words, participants who had greater hyperarousal scores also had reduced memory-related
activity during the encoding of trauma-related pictures. This finding is consistent
with evidence for an inverted U-shaped function in the hippocampus as a function of
stress (Nadel and Jacobs, 1998) and provides a possible explanation for the non-specific
effects observed in the tasks assessing emotional memory for trauma-related cues and
their undifferentiated impact on goal-relevant processing when presented as task-irrelevant
distraction. Consistent with the role of the initial arousal in these effects, PTSD
patients also showed relatively greater activity for forgotten items, which may be
linked to AMY hyperactivity leading to later forgetting of those items (Hayes et al.,
2011).
Conclusion
In summary, available evidence from investigations of PTSD patients points to general
and specific emotional and cognitive disturbances that are linked to alterations in
the neural circuitry underlying emotion-cognition interactions. This evidence suggests
that reduction of AMY and HC signals for trauma-related cues may underlie non-specific
encoding of gist-based representations instead of specific and detailed contextual
details of the trauma-related memories. This, in turn, may be linked to symptoms of
hypervigilance and non-specific responses to trauma-related distraction, which contributes
to the maintenance of a hyperarousal state (Figure 1D). This evidence also highlights
the importance of investigating both the enhancing and the impairing effects of emotion,
in understanding the changes associated with affective disorders, where both effects
are intensified. Collectively, these findings point to the importance of investigating
both of these opposing effects of emotion within the same clinical group, to complement
similar approaches in healthy participant concomitantly investigating the enhancing
and impairing effects of emotion on cognitive processes (Shafer and Dolcos, 2012;
Dolcos et al., 2013).