Inviting an author to review:
Find an author and click ‘Invite to review selected article’ near their name.
Search for authorsSearch for similar articles
141
views
0
recommends
+1 Recommend
0 collections
    0
    shares
      • Record: found
      • Abstract: found
      • Article: found
      Is Open Access

      Real-time dialogue between experimenters and dreamers during REM sleep

      research-article

      Read this article at

      Bookmark
          There is no author summary for this article yet. Authors can add summaries to their articles on ScienceOpen to make them more accessible to a non-specialist audience.

          SUMMARY

          Dreams take us to a different reality, a hallucinatory world that feels as real as any waking experience. These often-bizarre episodes are emblematic of human sleep but have yet to be adequately explained. Retrospective dream reports are subject to distortion and forgetting, presenting a fundamental challenge for neuroscientific studies of dreaming. Here we show that individuals who are asleep and in the midst of a lucid dream (aware of the fact that they are currently dreaming) can perceive questions from an experimenter and provide answers using electrophysiological signals. We implemented our procedures for two-way communication during polysomnographically verified rapid-eye-movement (REM) sleep in 36 individuals. Some had minimal prior experience with lucid dreaming, others were frequent lucid dreamers, and one was a patient with narcolepsy who had frequent lucid dreams. During REM sleep, these individuals exhibited various capabilities, including performing veridical perceptual analysis of novel information, maintaining information in working memory, computing simple answers, and expressing volitional replies. Their responses included distinctive eye movements and selective facial muscle contractions, constituting correctly answered questions on 29 occasions across 6 of the individuals tested. These repeated observations of interactive dreaming, documented by four independent laboratory groups, demonstrate that phenomenological and cognitive characteristics of dreaming can be interrogated in real time. This relatively unexplored communication channel can enable a variety of practical applications and a new strategy for the empirical exploration of dreams.

          Graphical Abstract

          In Brief

          Scientific investigations of dreaming have been hampered by the delay between a dream and when people report on their dream, and by a change in state from sleep to wake. To overcome this problem, Konkoly et al. show that individuals in REM sleep can perceive and answer an experimenter’s questions, allowing for real-time communication about a dream.

          Related collections

          Most cited references73

          • Record: found
          • Abstract: found
          • Article: not found

          The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research

          Despite the prevalence of sleep complaints among psychiatric patients, few questionnaires have been specifically designed to measure sleep quality in clinical populations. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) is a self-rated questionnaire which assesses sleep quality and disturbances over a 1-month time interval. Nineteen individual items generate seven "component" scores: subjective sleep quality, sleep latency, sleep duration, habitual sleep efficiency, sleep disturbances, use of sleeping medication, and daytime dysfunction. The sum of scores for these seven components yields one global score. Clinical and clinimetric properties of the PSQI were assessed over an 18-month period with "good" sleepers (healthy subjects, n = 52) and "poor" sleepers (depressed patients, n = 54; sleep-disorder patients, n = 62). Acceptable measures of internal homogeneity, consistency (test-retest reliability), and validity were obtained. A global PSQI score greater than 5 yielded a diagnostic sensitivity of 89.6% and specificity of 86.5% (kappa = 0.75, p less than 0.001) in distinguishing good and poor sleepers. The clinimetric and clinical properties of the PSQI suggest its utility both in psychiatric clinical practice and research activities.
            Bookmark
            • Record: found
            • Abstract: found
            • Article: not found

            The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research.

            Despite the prevalence of sleep complaints among psychiatric patients, few questionnaires have been specifically designed to measure sleep quality in clinical populations. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) is a self-rated questionnaire which assesses sleep quality and disturbances over a 1-month time interval. Nineteen individual items generate seven "component" scores: subjective sleep quality, sleep latency, sleep duration, habitual sleep efficiency, sleep disturbances, use of sleeping medication, and daytime dysfunction. The sum of scores for these seven components yields one global score. Clinical and clinimetric properties of the PSQI were assessed over an 18-month period with "good" sleepers (healthy subjects, n = 52) and "poor" sleepers (depressed patients, n = 54; sleep-disorder patients, n = 62). Acceptable measures of internal homogeneity, consistency (test-retest reliability), and validity were obtained. A global PSQI score greater than 5 yielded a diagnostic sensitivity of 89.6% and specificity of 86.5% (kappa = 0.75, p less than 0.001) in distinguishing good and poor sleepers. The clinimetric and clinical properties of the PSQI suggest its utility both in psychiatric clinical practice and research activities.
              Bookmark
              • Record: found
              • Abstract: found
              • Article: not found

              Local sleep and learning.

              Human sleep is a global state whose functions remain unclear. During much of sleep, cortical neurons undergo slow oscillations in membrane potential, which appear in electroencephalograms as slow wave activity (SWA) of <4 Hz. The amount of SWA is homeostatically regulated, increasing after wakefulness and returning to baseline during sleep. It has been suggested that SWA homeostasis may reflect synaptic changes underlying a cellular need for sleep. If this were so, inducing local synaptic changes should induce local SWA changes, and these should benefit neural function. Here we show that sleep homeostasis indeed has a local component, which can be triggered by a learning task involving specific brain regions. Furthermore, we show that the local increase in SWA after learning correlates with improved performance of the task after sleep. Thus, sleep homeostasis can be induced on a local level and can benefit performance.
                Bookmark

                Author and article information

                Journal
                9107782
                8548
                Curr Biol
                Curr Biol
                Current biology : CB
                0960-9822
                1879-0445
                17 May 2021
                18 February 2021
                12 April 2021
                28 May 2021
                : 31
                : 7
                : 1417-1427.e6
                Affiliations
                [1 ]Department of Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience Program, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA
                [2 ]Institute of Cognitive Science, Osnabrück University, Osnabrück, Germany
                [3 ]Institute of Sleep and Dream Technologies, Hamburg, Germany
                [4 ]Institut du Cerveau - Paris Brain Institute - ICM, Sorbonne Université, Inserm, CNRS, Paris, France
                [5 ]Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, the Netherlands
                [6 ]Department of Medical and Surgical Sciences, University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy
                [7 ]Department of Psychology, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA
                [8 ]NextSense, Inc., Mountain View, CA, USA
                [9 ]AP-HP, Pitié -Salpêtrière Hospital, Sleep Disorders Department, Paris, France
                [10 ]Twitter: @kap101
                [11 ]These authors contributed equally
                [12 ]These authors contributed equally
                [13 ]Lead contact
                Author notes

                AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS

                Conceptual, K.R.K., K.A., E.C., A.M., J.G., R.M., J.B.B., J.-B.M., G.P., I.A., D.O., M.D., and K.A.P.; Investigation, K.R.K., K.A., E.C., A.M., J.G., B.C., C.Y.M., and B.T.; Data Curation, K.R.K., K.A., E.C., A.M., S.W., N.W.W., F.D.W., and S.L.-S.; Visualization, K.R.K., K.A., E.C., A.M., and K.A.P.; Writing, K.R.K., K.A., A.M., B.T., D.O., M.D., and K.A.P.

                [* ]Correspondence: kap@ 123456northwestern.edu
                Article
                NIHMS1704833
                10.1016/j.cub.2021.01.026
                8162929
                33607035
                39979176-270b-4ac2-9b9c-a5191925a3f4

                This is an open access article under the CC BY license ( http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).

                History
                Categories
                Article

                Life sciences
                Life sciences

                Comments

                Comment on this article

                scite_
                0
                0
                0
                0
                Smart Citations
                0
                0
                0
                0
                Citing PublicationsSupportingMentioningContrasting
                View Citations

                See how this article has been cited at scite.ai

                scite shows how a scientific paper has been cited by providing the context of the citation, a classification describing whether it supports, mentions, or contrasts the cited claim, and a label indicating in which section the citation was made.

                Similar content171

                Cited by28

                Most referenced authors520