123
views
0
recommends
+1 Recommend
0 collections
    0
    shares
      • Record: found
      • Abstract: found
      • Article: found
      Is Open Access

      Poor replication validity of biomedical association studies reported by newspapers

      research-article

      Read this article at

      Bookmark
          There is no author summary for this article yet. Authors can add summaries to their articles on ScienceOpen to make them more accessible to a non-specialist audience.

          Abstract

          Objective

          To investigate the replication validity of biomedical association studies covered by newspapers.

          Methods

          We used a database of 4723 primary studies included in 306 meta-analysis articles. These studies associated a risk factor with a disease in three biomedical domains, psychiatry, neurology and four somatic diseases. They were classified into a lifestyle category (e.g. smoking) and a non-lifestyle category (e.g. genetic risk). Using the database Dow Jones Factiva, we investigated the newspaper coverage of each study. Their replication validity was assessed using a comparison with their corresponding meta-analyses.

          Results

          Among the 5029 articles of our database, 156 primary studies (of which 63 were lifestyle studies) and 5 meta-analysis articles were reported in 1561 newspaper articles. The percentage of covered studies and the number of newspaper articles per study strongly increased with the impact factor of the journal that published each scientific study. Newspapers almost equally covered initial (5/39 12.8%) and subsequent (58/600 9.7%) lifestyle studies. In contrast, initial non-lifestyle studies were covered more often (48/366 13.1%) than subsequent ones (45/3718 1.2%). Newspapers never covered initial studies reporting null findings and rarely reported subsequent null observations. Only 48.7% of the 156 studies reported by newspapers were confirmed by the corresponding meta-analyses. Initial non-lifestyle studies were less often confirmed (16/48) than subsequent ones (29/45) and than lifestyle studies (31/63). Psychiatric studies covered by newspapers were less often confirmed (10/38) than the neurological (26/41) or somatic (40/77) ones. This is correlated to an even larger coverage of initial studies in psychiatry. Whereas 234 newspaper articles covered the 35 initial studies that were later disconfirmed, only four press articles covered a subsequent null finding and mentioned the refutation of an initial claim.

          Conclusion

          Journalists preferentially cover initial findings although they are often contradicted by meta-analyses and rarely inform the public when they are disconfirmed.

          Related collections

          Most cited references29

          • Record: found
          • Abstract: found
          • Article: not found

          Evaluating dopamine reward pathway in ADHD: clinical implications.

          Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)--characterized by symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity--is the most prevalent childhood psychiatric disorder that frequently persists into adulthood, and there is increasing evidence of reward-motivation deficits in this disorder. To evaluate biological bases that might underlie a reward/motivation deficit by imaging key components of the brain dopamine reward pathway (mesoaccumbens). We used positron emission tomography to measure dopamine synaptic markers (transporters and D(2)/D(3) receptors) in 53 nonmedicated adults with ADHD and 44 healthy controls between 2001-2009 at Brookhaven National Laboratory. We measured specific binding of positron emission tomographic radioligands for dopamine transporters (DAT) using [(11)C]cocaine and for D(2)/D(3) receptors using [(11)C]raclopride, quantified as binding potential (distribution volume ratio -1). For both ligands, statistical parametric mapping showed that specific binding was lower in ADHD than in controls (threshold for significance set at P < .005) in regions of the dopamine reward pathway in the left side of the brain. Region-of-interest analyses corroborated these findings. The mean (95% confidence interval [CI] of mean difference) for DAT in the nucleus accumbens for controls was 0.71 vs 0.63 for those with ADHD (95% CI, 0.03-0.13, P = .004) and in the midbrain for controls was 0.16 vs 0.09 for those with ADHD (95% CI, 0.03-0.12; P < or = .001); for D(2)/D(3) receptors, the mean accumbens for controls was 2.85 vs 2.68 for those with ADHD (95% CI, 0.06-0.30, P = .004); and in the midbrain, it was for controls 0.28 vs 0.18 for those with ADHD (95% CI, 0.02-0.17, P = .01). The analysis also corroborated differences in the left caudate: the mean DAT for controls was 0.66 vs 0.53 for those with ADHD (95% CI, 0.04-0.22; P = .003) and the mean D(2)/D(3) for controls was 2.80 vs 2.47 for those with ADHD (95% CI, 0.10-0.56; P = .005) and differences in D(2)/D(3) in the hypothalamic region, with controls having a mean of 0.12 vs 0.05 for those with ADHD (95% CI, 0.02-0.12; P = .004). Ratings of attention correlated with D(2)/D(3) in the accumbens (r = 0.35; 95% CI, 0.15-0.52; P = .001), midbrain (r = 0.35; 95% CI, 0.14-0.52; P = .001), caudate (r = 0.32; 95% CI, 0.11-0.50; P = .003), and hypothalamic (r = 0.31; CI, 0.10-0.49; P = .003) regions and with DAT in the midbrain (r = 0.37; 95% CI, 0.16-0.53; P < or = .001). A reduction in dopamine synaptic markers associated with symptoms of inattention was shown in the dopamine reward pathway of participants with ADHD.
            Bookmark
            • Record: found
            • Abstract: found
            • Article: not found

            Coverage by the news media of the benefits and risks of medications.

            The news media are an important source of information about new medical treatments, but there is concern that some coverage may be inaccurate and overly enthusiastic. We studied coverage by U.S. news media of the benefits and risks of three medications that are used to prevent major diseases. The medications were pravastatin, a cholesterol-lowering drug for the prevention of cardiovascular disease; alendronate, a bisphosphonate for the treatment and prevention of osteoporosis; and aspirin, which is used for the prevention of cardiovascular disease. We analyzed a systematic probability sample of 180 newspaper articles (60 for each drug) and 27 television reports that appeared between 1994 and 1998. Of the 207 stories, 83 (40 percent) did not report benefits quantitatively. Of the 124 that did, 103 (83 percent) reported relative benefits only, 3 (2 percent) absolute benefits only, and 18 (15 percent) both absolute and relative benefits. Of the 207 stories, 98 (47 percent) mentioned potential harm to patients, and only 63 (30 percent) mentioned costs. Of the 170 stories citing an expert or a scientific study, 85 (50 percent) cited at least one expert or study with a financial tie to a manufacturer of the drug that had been disclosed in the scientific literature. These ties were disclosed in only 33 (39 percent) of the 85 stories. News-media stories about medications may include inadequate or incomplete information about the benefits, risks, and costs of the drugs as well as the financial ties between study groups or experts and pharmaceutical manufacturers.
              Bookmark
              • Record: found
              • Abstract: found
              • Article: found
              Is Open Access

              Misrepresentation of Randomized Controlled Trials in Press Releases and News Coverage: A Cohort Study

              Introduction The media play an important role in the dissemination of findings from health research. More than half of US adults report that they follow health news closely [1]. Further, 90% of the general public gets most of its information about science from the mass media [2]. Press releases are a major source of information for one-third of medical reports in US newspapers [3]. Press releases are widely used by the medical researchers to attract favorable media attention [4]–[6] and to promote their research [7]–[9]. A press release should provide journalists with the basic information needed to develop a news story and publish it in the mass media. Randomized controlled trials (RCTs) are considered the gold standard for therapeutic evaluation [10]. Adequate and undistorted communication of the findings from RCTs is essential for physicians, researchers, and patients because it allows for efficient uptake of research into clinical practice [11]. Theoretically, in reports of RCTs published in peer-reviewed journals, the data should speak for themselves. However, a recent study showed that research findings can be distorted in published articles, by the use of “spin,” which is defined as specific reporting emphasizing the beneficial effect of the experimental treatment [12]. The types of distorted presentation or “spin” are diverse, with, for example, a particular focus on statistically significant results (within-group comparison, subgroup analyses, and secondary outcomes) or an inadequate interpretation of nonstatistically significant differences as demonstrating equivalence in treatment effectiveness or lack of difference in adverse events. We aimed to (1) evaluate the presence of “spin” in press releases and associated media coverage and (2) evaluate whether findings of RCTs contained within press releases and media coverage are misinterpreted. Methods Selection of Press Releases, Related Scientific Articles, and News Items We identified all press releases indexed in EurekAlert! (online free database for science press releases; www.eurekalert.org) between December 1, 2009, and March 31, 2010, using the following search strategy: topic “medicine and health,” type of release “research news,” keyword: random* [4],[13]. We included press releases for published results of two-arm, parallel-group RCTs defined as prospective studies assessing health care interventions in human participants. To have a homogeneous sample, we excluded press releases for equivalence or noninferiority, cross-over, cluster, and multiple-arm trials; follow-up studies; press releases not reported in English; and those about more than one study. Duplicate press releases (i.e., press releases published more than once in the database) were systematically searched and excluded. The title and full text of all retrieved press releases were screened by one reviewer to exclude any non-eligible press releases. We obtained a copy of the scientific article related to the press release from (1) the direct link or full reference citation reported in the press release, if available; or (2) the PubMed single citation matcher indicating the year of publication, journal, and author's name. Each retrieved scientific article (abstract and full text) was assessed by the same reader to confirm eligibility. Finally, for all selected press releases, we systematically searched for related news items in the “general news” library of LEXIS-NEXIS using (1) the name of the disease; (2) the treatment being evaluated, and, if needed, the name of the first or second author. All news related to the articles or press releases were retrieved, and we selected the news that had the highest number of words dedicated to the selected study. Data Abstraction Data were abstracted from the press release, news items, and the related published scientific article. For this purpose, we developed a standardized data-abstraction form using previous work on the same topics [12]–[14]. The data-abstraction form and details about the methods is available in Texts S1 and S2. The data-abstraction form was preliminarily tested by two of the reviewers with a sample of 15 press releases and original articles indexed in January 2008. The data that involved some subjectivity, such as the type of “spin” were abstracted by two independent reviewers, with discrepancies resolved by consensus. Other data were evaluated by a single reviewer. The concordance between the two reviewers for the assessment of “spin” is reported in Text S3; the mean kappa coefficient for “spin” was 0.56 (range 0.43–0.69). We systematically extracted data related to the characteristics of (1) the RCT, (2) the press releases, and (3) the presence of “spin” in the article abstract conclusions, in the press release and, when available, in the news items. We defined “spin” as a specific reporting (intentional or unintentional) that emphasizes the beneficial effect of the experimental treatment. We used a classification of “spin” described in a previous work [12]. This classification was initially developed in the context of trials with a nonstatistically significant primary outcome. This classification was adapted for all RCTs. We considered “spin” as being a focus on statistically significant results (within-group comparison, secondary outcomes, subgroup analyses, modified population of analyses); an interpretation of statistically nonsignificant results for the primary outcomes as showing treatment equivalence or comparable effectiveness; or any inadequate claim of safety or emphasis of the beneficial effect of the treatment. Results Interpretation The RCT results were interpreted independently from three different sources: (1) from the full text of the scientific article, (2) from the press release, and (3) from the news items. For each source, different pairs of assessors independently evaluated the results of the RCT and achieved consensus. Assessment based on the scientific article relied on the results for the primary outcomes, secondary outcomes, and harm. For assessment of press releases, assessors were blinded to the authors of the press release, the content of the scientific article, and the journal of publication. For assessment of news items, assessors were blinded to the content of the press release and scientific article. All results reported represent the consensus of each pair of assessors. Interpreting the RCT results The trial results were interpreted independently by use of the same scale, from 1 to 5 [15]. According to this scale, the assessors had to indicate whether patients should (1) definitely get the experimental treatment evaluated, (2) probably get the experimental treatment evaluated, (3) decide for themselves (i.e., the article was neutral), (4) probably not get the experimental treatment evaluated, or (5) definitely not get the experimental treatment evaluated. If the interpretation of the RCT results was classified as 1 or 2, the experimental treatment was considered beneficial; 3, the trial results were neutral; 4 or 5, the experimental treatment was considered not beneficial. Definition of misinterpretation Misinterpretation was defined as the interpretation of the press release or news items differing from that based on the full-text article by at least one class according to the above three-class system of scores. Misinterpretation of the press release or news items could overestimate the treatment beneficial effect or underestimate the treatment effect. For example, an overestimation of the treatment beneficial effect in the press release or news items occurred when reading the published article led to rating the trial results as neutral, whereas reading the press release or new items led to rating the experimental treatment as beneficial. Statistical Analysis Data for quantitative variables are expressed with medians and IQRs. Data for qualitative variables are expressed with frequencies and percentages. We planned bivariate and multivariable analysis to identify factors associated with (1) “spin” in the press releases, (2) an overestimation of the beneficial effect of the experimental treatment from press releases, (3) “spin” in the news items, and (4) an overestimation of the beneficial effect of the experimental treatment from news items. For bivariate analysis, we used the chi-square or Fisher exact test for categorical data and the Student t-test for quantitative data. For the multivariable analysis, we performed a Poisson regression with robust error variance [16] with a bootstrap model selection variable method [17] to assess all relevant variables. We used 1,000 bootstrap samples. Variables with p 0.05 as demonstrating equivalence (7%); inappropriate extrapolation (9%); focus on statistically significant results such as subgroup analyses (6%), within-group comparisons (9%), and secondary outcomes (4%); or inadequate claim of safety (6%). 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001308.t001 Table 1 General characteristics of articles. Characteristics Subcharacteristics n = 70 (%) Type of journal, n (%) General medical journal 32 (46) Specialized medical journal 38 (54) Funding source, n (%) Profit or both profit and nonprofit 33 (47) None or nonprofit 33 (47) Not reported 4 (6) Sample size median; [IQR]; (min–max) 112; [54–435]; (16–94,370) Experimental treatment, n (%) Drug 36 (51) Surgery/procedure 9 (13) Device 5 (7) Therapeutic strategy 7 (10) Participative intervention 12 (17) Other 1 (1) Comparator, n (%) Placebo 29 (41) Active treatment 32(46) Other 9 (13) Primary outcomes clearly identified, n (%) 61(87) Type of primary outcomes, n (%) Efficacy 61 (87) Safety 1 (1) Both 4 (6) Unclear 4 (6) Primary outcomes reported adequately, n (%)a 56 (80) Results of primary outcomes, n (%) All statistically significant 34 (49) All statistically nonsignificant 24 (34) Some statistically significant/some not 11 (16) Unclear 1 (1) At least one “spin” 28 (40) Type of “spin”b No acknowledgment of nonstatistically significant primary outcome 14 (20) Claiming equivalence when results failed to demonstrate a statistically significant difference 5 (7) Focus on positive secondary outcome 3 (4) Focus on inappropriate subgroup 4 (6) Focus on within-group (or over-all within) comparison 6 (9) Nonstatistically significant outcome reported as if they were significant 3 (4) Ignored data of safety 1 (1) Inadequate claim of safety 4 (6) Inappropriate extrapolation 6 (9) Other 5 (7) a Adequately, with effect size and precision or treatment effect in each arm with precision. b Numbers do not add up as the types of “spin” were not mutually exclusive. Characteristics of Press Releases The general characteristics of press releases are in Table 2: 57% were written by a press officer; half provided easy access to the research article that had been press released (i.e., a direct link or the full reference) and 25 (36%) reported the funding source. The results for primary outcomes were reported with words only in 29 (41%) press releases. Safety was mentioned in 24 (34%) and quantified in 14 (20%); the study limitations were reported in ten (14%). A total of 58 (83%) press releases contained quotations from authors or editors of the article. In 30 (52%), the interview reported results with emphasis, such as “this work paves the way for further study,” or in 22 (38%) with moderation, such as “further investigation is needed to establish (…).” Quotations from the article were included in 22 (31%) of the releases. In 11 (50%), the quotations reported results with emphasis, such as “clinical findings are indeed very encouraging, said Dr…”), or in seven (32%) with moderation. 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001308.t002 Table 2 General characteristics of press releases. Characteristics Subcharacteristics n = 70 (%) Origin, n (%) Press officer 40 (57) Industry or institution 30 (43) Easy access to full article, (i.e., direct link or the full reference) n (%) 36 (51) Funding reported, n (%) 25 (36) Design reported, n (%) 70 (100) Sample size reported, n (%) 65 (93) Length of follow-up reported, n (%) 46 (66) Primary outcomes reported, n (%) In words only 29 (41) Per arms 30 (43) With effect size 17 (24) Safety reported, n (%) Mentioned 24 (34) Quantified 14 (20) Limits reported, n (%) 10 (14) Interview included, n (%) Authors only 40 (57) Experts or editorialists only 6 (9) Both 12 (17) Article quotation reported, n (%) 22 (31) At least one type of “spin” 33 (47) Type of “spin”a No acknowledgment of nonstatistically significant primary outcome 13 (19) Claiming equivalence when results failed to demonstrate a statistically significant difference 7 (10) Focus on positive secondary outcome 5 (7) Focus on inappropriate subgroup 4 (5) Focus on within-group (or over-all within) comparison 11 (16) Nonstatistically significant outcome reported as if they were significant 5 (7) Ignored data of safety 3 (4) Inadequate claim of safety 5 (7) Inappropriate extrapolation 6 (9) Other “spin” 2 (3) a Numbers do not add up as the types of “spin” were not mutually exclusive. About half of the press releases (33; 47%) had at least one type of “spin” (Table 2). Factors Associated with “Spin” in Press Releases From bivariate analysis (Table 3), “spin” in press releases was more frequent in trials published in a specialized journal (58% versus 34% in a general journal; p = 0.05), trials with small sample size (i.e., <112) (63% versus 31%; p = 0.008), and trials with “spin” in the scientific article abstract conclusion (93%, yes, versus 17%, no; p<0.001). The presence of “spin” in the press release was not associated with funding source (45% profit versus 49% other: p = 0.8), author of the press release (48% press officer versus 47% other; p = 0.9), the experimental treatment (47% drug versus 47% other; p = 1.0) or results of the primary outcome (46% all nonstatistically significant versus 48% other; p = 0.9). In multivariable analysis including all variables with p<0.25 in the bivariate analysis (i.e., journal, “spin” in the abstract conclusion, and sample size), the only factor associated with “spin” in the press release was “spin” in the scientific article abstract conclusions (RR = 5.6, 95% CI 2.8–11.0, p<0.001) (Text S5). 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001308.t003 Table 3 Bivariate analysis of factors associated with and “spin” in the press releases. Characteristics Subcharacteristics “Spin” in Press Release n/Total n (%) p-Value Journal General 11/32 (34) 0.05 Specialized 22/38 (58) — Funding source Profit 15/33 (45) 0.8 Nonprofit or not reported 18/37 (49) — Sample size <112 22/35 (63) 0.008 ≥112 11/35 (31) — Experimental treatment Drug 17/36 (47) 1.0 Other 16/34 (47) — Results of primary outcome(s) All nonstatistically significant 11/24 (46) 0.9 Other 22/46 (48) — Authors of press release Press officer 19/40 (48) 0.9 Other 14/30 (47) — “Spin” in abstract conclusion Yes 26/28 (93) <0.001 No 7/42 (17) — Interpretation of the Trial Results from Press Releases For the interpretation based on the full-text scientific articles, for 38 articles (54%), the experimental treatment was considered beneficial, 18 (26%) neutral, and 14 (20%) not beneficial. In contrast, for the interpretation based on press releases, for 55 releases (79%), the experimental treatment was considered beneficial, two (3%) neutral, and 13 (18%) not beneficial. The results were misinterpreted in 22 press releases (31%); for 19 (86%), the assessors overestimated the benefit of the experimental treatment from the press release and for three (14%), they underestimated the benefit of the experimental treatment from the press release. As shown in Table 4, on the basis of press releases, the benefit of the experimental treatment was overestimated more often for trial results published in a specialized journal rather than in a general medical journal (45% versus 6%; p<0.001), for trials with a small rather than large sample size (46% versus 9%; p<0.001), for trials with nonstatistically rather than significant primary outcomes (42% versus20%; p = 0.05), and for trials with “spin” rather than without “spin” in the press release (48% versus 8%; p<0.001). These results did not differ significantly by funding source, author of the press release, or type of experimental treatment. 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001308.t004 Table 4 Bivariate analysis of factors associated with an overestimation of the benefit of the experimental treatment from the press releases as compared with the interpretation from articles. Characteristics Subcharacteristics Overestimation of the Benefit of the Experimental Treatment n/Total n (%) p-Value Journal General 2/32 (6) <0.001 Specialized 17/38 (45) Funding source Profit 7/33 (21) 0.3 Nonprofit or not reported 12/37 (32) Sample size n<112 16/35 (46) <0.001 n≥112 3/35 (9) Experimental treatment Drug 11/36 (31) 0.5 Other 8/34 (24) Results of primary outcome(s) All nonstatistically significant 10/24 (42) 0.05 Other 9/46 (20) Authors of press release Press officer 10/40 (25) 0.6 Other 9/30 (30) “Spin” in press releases Yes 16/33 (48) <0.001 No 3/37 (8) “Spin” and Interpretation of the News For a sample of 41 RCTS we retrieved the scientific article, the press release, and any news items. “Spin” was identified in 17 (41%) abstracts, 19 (46%) press releases, and 21 (51%) news items. Figure 2 describes the reporting of “spin” in abstracts, press releases, and news items. For the 17 abstracts reported with “spin”, 16 press releases and related news items featured the same “spin.” For the 24 abstracts without “spin,” only three press releases featured “spin,” which was subsequently reported in the related news items. Examples of “spin” in the abstract and related press releases and news items are in Figure 3. The factors associated with “spin” in the news were specialty journals (67% versus 35%; p = 0.04), small sample size (68% versus 32%; p = 0.02), “spin” in abstract (100% versus 5%; p<0.001), and “spin” in the press release (100% versus 13%; p<0.001) (Text S6). 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001308.g002 Figure 2 “Spin” in abstract conclusions, press releases, and news items. 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001308.g003 Figure 3 Examples of “spin” in abstracts, in press releases, and in related news items. Overall, the assessors overestimated the benefit of the experimental treatment from the news for 10 (24%) reports. Factors associated with overestimation of the beneficial effect of the treatment from the news items were small sample size (41% versus 5%, p = 0.01), and “spin” in the news (43% versus 5%, p = 0.009) (Text S7). Discussion Our results highlight a tendency for press releases and the associated media coverage of RCTs to place emphasis on the beneficial effects of experimental treatments. This tendency is probably related to the presence of “spin” in conclusions of the scientific article's abstract. This tendency, in conjunction with other well-known biases such as publication bias, selective reporting of outcomes, and lack of external validity, may be responsible for an important gap between the public perception of the beneficial effect and the real effect of the treatment studied. Previous studies have highlighted the importance of press releases for results communication and dissemination [4],[5]. Indeed, as a direct means of communication between medical journals and the media, press releases provide an opportunity for journals to influence how the research is translated into news [4]. The press release is essential when considering the impact of press coverage by the media on health care utilization, clinical practice, and researchers' behavior [1]. This influence has been clearly shown in a quasi-experimental study evaluating the impact of media coverage [11]. The authors compared the number of scientific citations of articles published in the New England Journal of Medicine that were covered by the New York Times to similar articles that were not covered. They also performed this comparison during a 3-mo period when the New York Times was on strike; the New York Times continued to print an “edition of record” but did not sell copies to the public because of the strike. The authors demonstrated that the high citation of articles covered by the New York Times was not present during the strike. Consequently, the high citation was related to the media coverage, not the importance of the research [18]. A Cochrane systematic review highlighted the impact of the mass media on health services utilization [19]. It showed a consistent effect after planned campaigns and unplanned coverage. Another study showed a clear association of the media coverage of invasive group A streptococcal (GAS) disease and testing for GAS in pediatric emergency departments, with an important increase in the prescription of rapid tests for GAS in pediatric emergency departments concomitant with a peak in media attention, despite no increase in the number of children presenting symptoms that might warrant such testing [20]. Unfortunately, as shown in our study, and previous work the quality of media reports is questionable. An assessment of the reporting of medical news in the mainstream media highlighted the inadequate accuracy and balance of the news media in reporting medical science [21]–[23]. The criticisms of the mainstream media also applied to press releases. Woloshin et al., in evaluating press releases issued by 20 academic medical centers, showed that the releases frequently promoted preliminary research without giving basic details or the cautions needed to judge the meaning, relevance, or validation of the science (42% of press releases evaluated in this study did not provide any relevant caveats, and 90% about animal or laboratory studies lacked caveats about extrapolating results to humans) [13]. Furthermore, press releases tended to overstate the importance of the research, 29% were rated as exaggerating the findings' importance and 26% of investigator quotes were considered to overstate the research importance [13]. Recently, a study showed that the quality of press releases influenced subsequent media coverage content [24]. Of course, press releases are not meant to be condensed versions of scientific papers; they are meant to summarize the most important findings, contextualize these finding for journalists, and provide contact details for authors and quotes. By being condensed, they always lack details that are contained in the papers. The use of “spin” or a particular emphasis could be a way to increase the interest of journalists and subsequent citations in the peer-reviewed literature. However, this situation becomes problematic if it modifies readers' interpretation of research findings. Our results add to these previous studies by showing the link between the distorted presentation and interpretation of the results in scientific articles and the distorted content and interpretation of press releases. These findings raise the issue of the quality of the peer review process and highlight the importance of this process for disseminating accurate research results. Our study has several limitations. Firstly, our sample included only published reports of RCTs with a press release indexed in the Eurekalert! database within a 4 mo period, and reported in English; this sample may not be representative of all press releases of RCT results. In fact, half of the press releases selected were written by press officers of medical journals with a high impact factor. Other sources of press releases exist on industry websites, medical journal websites, or other databases for journalists. However, the Eurekalert! database is one of the most important sources of freely available press releases, and most research published on press releases has used this database. Further, there is no reason to believe that the selection of the sample over only 4 mo would bias the results. Secondly, RCTs represent only a small part of the medical literature and the findings may not apply to media reporting of medical or scientific research as a whole. Thirdly, we searched for “spin” only in the article abstract conclusions, not in the entire published article. Consequently, we are not able to determine whether “spin” in the press release was the same as the “spin” in the whole article. We chose the abstract conclusions because it is the most accessible section of an article. Readers often base their initial assessment of a trial on the information reported in an abstract conclusion, and in some geographic areas, the abstract of an RCT report may be all that health professionals have easy access to [25],[26]. Fourthly, the content analysis and the interpretation coding were subjective [27]. However, two independent reviewers performed this assessment with consensus. Fifthly, we focused on articles and press releases of RCT results. We did not evaluate press releases for other study designs or proceedings of conferences. In conclusion, previous work showed that exaggerated and inappropriate coverage of research findings in the news media is linked to inappropriate reporting of press releases. Our study adds to these results showing that “spin” in press releases and the news is related to the presence of “spin” in the published article, namely the abstract conclusions. Additionally, our work highlights that this inappropriate reporting could bias readers' interpretation of research results. Consequently, reviewers and editors of published articles have an important role to play in the dissemination of research findings and should be particularly aware of the need to ensure that the conclusions reported are an appropriate reflection of the trial findings and do not overinterpret or misinterpret the results. Supporting Information Text S1 Data abstraction form. (DOC) Click here for additional data file. Text S2 Details related to the method. (DOC) Click here for additional data file. Text S3 Kappa coefficient or agreement percentage for the assessment of “spin” in press releases and in articles. (DOC) Click here for additional data file. Text S4 List of press releases and published articles examined. (DOC) Click here for additional data file. Text S5 Multivariate analysis of factors associated with “spin” in press releases. (DOC) Click here for additional data file. Text S6 Bivariate analysis of factors associated with “spin” in news items (n = 41). (DOC) Click here for additional data file. Text S7 Bivariate analysis of factors associated with an overestimation of the benefit of the experimental treatment from the news as compared with the interpretation from the article abstract conclusions (n = 41). (DOC) Click here for additional data file.
                Bookmark

                Author and article information

                Contributors
                Role: Editor
                Journal
                PLoS One
                PLoS ONE
                plos
                plosone
                PLoS ONE
                Public Library of Science (San Francisco, CA USA )
                1932-6203
                21 February 2017
                2017
                : 12
                : 2
                : e0172650
                Affiliations
                [1 ]Centre Emile Durkheim, CNRS UMR5116 at Université de Bordeaux, Pessac, France
                [2 ]Institute of Neurodegenerative Diseases, CNRS UMR5293 at Université de Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France
                Tilburg University, NETHERLANDS
                Author notes

                Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

                • Conceptualization: EDM AS TB FG.

                • Data curation: EDM TB FG.

                • Formal analysis: EDM TB FG.

                • Investigation: EDM FG.

                • Methodology: EDM FG.

                • Project administration: EDM AS TB FG.

                • Supervision: AS TB FG.

                • Validation: FG.

                • Visualization: EDM TB FG.

                • Writing – original draft: EDM FG.

                • Writing – review & editing: EDM AS TB FG.

                Author information
                http://orcid.org/0000-0002-6011-8428
                Article
                PONE-D-16-42686
                10.1371/journal.pone.0172650
                5319681
                28222122
                2630ec80-0f4a-44e6-ab8c-56b3341c2aab
                © 2017 Dumas-Mallet et al

                This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

                History
                : 27 October 2016
                : 7 February 2017
                Page count
                Figures: 3, Tables: 2, Pages: 15
                Funding
                The authors received no specific funding for this work.
                Categories
                Research Article
                Research and Analysis Methods
                Mathematical and Statistical Techniques
                Statistical Methods
                Meta-Analysis
                Physical Sciences
                Mathematics
                Statistics (Mathematics)
                Statistical Methods
                Meta-Analysis
                Biology and Life Sciences
                Neuroscience
                Developmental Neuroscience
                Neurodevelopmental Disorders
                Adhd
                Medicine and Health Sciences
                Neurology
                Neurodevelopmental Disorders
                Adhd
                Medicine and Health Sciences
                Mental Health and Psychiatry
                Neuropsychiatric Disorders
                Adhd
                Medicine and Health Sciences
                Mental Health and Psychiatry
                Medicine and Health Sciences
                Health Care
                Health Risk Analysis
                Biology and Life Sciences
                Biochemistry
                Biomarkers
                Medicine and Health Sciences
                Oncology
                Cancers and Neoplasms
                Breast Tumors
                Breast Cancer
                Research and Analysis Methods
                Database and Informatics Methods
                Database Searching
                People and Places
                Population Groupings
                Professions
                Scientists
                Custom metadata
                All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.

                Uncategorized
                Uncategorized

                Comments

                Comment on this article