9
views
0
recommends
+1 Recommend
0 collections
    0
    shares
      • Record: found
      • Abstract: found
      • Article: found
      Is Open Access

      Commentary: Autism: A model of neurodevelopmental diversity informed by genomics

      article-commentary

      Read this article at

          There is no author summary for this article yet. Authors can add summaries to their articles on ScienceOpen to make them more accessible to a non-specialist audience.

          Abstract

          In their paper, Chawner and Owen (1) present a genetic model for autism that outlines two contributory factors: (1) a social and adaptive continuum due to common genetic variation; and (2) a neurodevelopmental continuum due to rare genetic variation that presents itself as a continuum of impairment spanning from intellectual disability, through autism and ADHD, to schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. I applaud the authors on relating the main mechanisms of the model to the differing views between the neurodiversity community and that of the medical model regarding the nature of autism, as they pertain to different aspects of the phenotype and both being important for explaining variability in clinical presentations. The model itself is very similar to part of a more comprehensive model I previously proposed (2, 3) and although there are many similarities between the papers, it is worth noting some empirical differences with important ramifications. I will argue that their conceptualization is not supported by the current literature and that it contains an issue that limits its practical usefulness. I will conclude by presenting testable postulates arising from the two models which will allow future studies to empirically validate them. They write that the “neurodevelopmental continuum […] results in a diverse spectrum of outcomes,” referring to individual diagnoses under the neurodevelopmental umbrella. It does that through the effects of rare genetic mutations and environmental risk factors. As they operationalize it, the magnitude of the rare genetic burden determines which phenotype develops, and ultimately which diagnosis is received [see Figure 1 in (1)]. Conceptually, greater impairment is more closely associated with intellectual disability and autism than with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Although the apparent statistical associations of these features appear in the literature, there is an issue with this operationalization that can be illustrated with an example. Consider an individual with a rare genetic burden of a given magnitude (X inherited ) and a diagnosis of bipolar disorder. If that individual were to have a child with inherited said burden, but with additional de novo variants (X inherited + X de novo ) that child should be more likely to develop autism or ADHD than bipolar disorder. The idea that the type of condition one develops is contingent on the magnitude of rare genetic risk is not supported by empirical evidence. The conditions have partly independent genotypic (4–7), and neuroendophenotypic signatures (8, 9), suggesting that they also have partly different biological backgrounds, rather than them being part of a single continuum. A person with bipolar disorder can certainly have a lower IQ and greater “cognitive impairment” than someone with an autism diagnosis. The operationalization of the neurodevelopmental continuum alludes to a causative mechanism by which the magnitude of the rare genetic burden impacts specificity of diagnosis. This is empirically unlikely given the state of the literature, unless the continuum is a pseudo-unidimensional manifold rather than linear, and it therefore probably represents a statistical artifact. Furthermore, following the conceptualization of a neurodevelopmental continuum, the addition of a social-adaptive factor to the model is not without issues since the autistic phenotype (which also encompasses such traits) is already conceptualized along the first factor. Clearly, the second factor is conceptualized in order to accommodate the literature on the association between autism and common genetic variation. However, within the proposed model one cannot dissociate the autistic phenotypes residing within each of the factors (whether an autistic trait belongs to the social-adaptive or the neurodevelopmental continuum), greatly limiting the practical utility of the proposed model. Their operationalization can be contrasted with that of the pathogenetic triad (2, 3), which previously suggested that there is (1) natural variation in non-pathological traits (such as autistic or schizotypal) due to common genetic variation, and (2) a range of neurodevelopmental risk factors including, but not limited to, rare genetic variation. These risk factors negatively influence brain and cognitive development, and limit adaptive behaviors. Notably, adaptive behavior is conceptualized within a third factor that moderates the association between the first two factors in giving rise to a diagnosis. This is an important distinction since Chawner and Owen seem to conceptualize adaptive behavior within the first factor as “social-adaptive traits” (although, they do not formally operationalize it). These two factors additively influence the risk, and crucially, the first factor provides the model with disorder specificity (through common variant burden for each condition, not rare burden). Also, rather than the magnitude of neurodevelopmental risk factors affecting which condition develops (as in their model), it non-specifically determines the probability of fulfilling criteria for any one diagnosis (or multiple). Although the models are similar, there are subtle differences that give rise to different empirical predictions, each with testable postulates. In Table 1 present a few of these predictions, and the patterns in the existing and future literature that would favor one model or the other (some of which are already supported or undermined). Table 1 Testable postulates for differences between the operationalizations. Genomic neurodevelopmental model (1) Pathogenetic triad (2, 3) Specificity of diagnosis Which diagnosis one receives depends on the individual rare variant genomic profile. Individuals are less likely to have conditions or endo-/phenotypes that are further from each other along the neurodevelopmental continuum [e.g., unlikely co-occurrence of BD and ID; (10, 11)]. Which diagnoses one receives depends on the individual common variant genomic profile. The presence of multiple diagnoses Individuals should only be able to get one diagnosis (although they write “frequent co-morbidity,” one cannot be located in two positions along a single continuum and simultaneously have a high and low rare genetic burden). The main clinical difficulty lies in ascertaining between those that are close to each other along the neurodevelopmental continuum. A higher neuropathological burden is positively associated with the risk of any one diagnosis, and the number of co-occurring diagnoses. The higher the burden, the more of the different disorder-specific traits/personality types (autistic, schizotypal etc.) become maladaptive and fulfill diagnostic criteria (12, 13). Transgenerational inheritance pattern Which condition(s) one develops is less related to the traits or conditions of the parents, and instead depends on the magnitude of rare genetic burden and risk factors. The condition(s) one develops depends on which common variants, traits and conditions the parents have. Parents that have a higher magnitude of rare genetic variants and risk factors are more likely to have children with any, and multiple condition(s). Distribution of traits in the population [See Figure 2 in (1)] An intermediate rare and common burden do not additively give rise to an autistic-like phenotype (due to empty area of plot), implying a non-continuous distribution and a strictly non-linear additivity for common and rare variants. [See Figure 2 in (3); Y-axis conceptually inverted compared with (1)] The first and second factor are both continuously distributed in the population (14), with additivity for common and rare variants. BD, Bipolar Disorder; ID, Intellectual Disability. Author contributions The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.

          Related collections

          Most cited references14

          • Record: found
          • Abstract: found
          • Article: found

          Genetic relationship between five psychiatric disorders estimated from genome-wide SNPs.

          Most psychiatric disorders are moderately to highly heritable. The degree to which genetic variation is unique to individual disorders or shared across disorders is unclear. To examine shared genetic etiology, we use genome-wide genotype data from the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium (PGC) for cases and controls in schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, major depressive disorder, autism spectrum disorders (ASD) and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). We apply univariate and bivariate methods for the estimation of genetic variation within and covariation between disorders. SNPs explained 17-29% of the variance in liability. The genetic correlation calculated using common SNPs was high between schizophrenia and bipolar disorder (0.68 ± 0.04 s.e.), moderate between schizophrenia and major depressive disorder (0.43 ± 0.06 s.e.), bipolar disorder and major depressive disorder (0.47 ± 0.06 s.e.), and ADHD and major depressive disorder (0.32 ± 0.07 s.e.), low between schizophrenia and ASD (0.16 ± 0.06 s.e.) and non-significant for other pairs of disorders as well as between psychiatric disorders and the negative control of Crohn's disease. This empirical evidence of shared genetic etiology for psychiatric disorders can inform nosology and encourages the investigation of common pathophysiologies for related disorders.
            Bookmark
            • Record: found
            • Abstract: found
            • Article: not found

            Psychosis and autism as diametrical disorders of the social brain.

            Autistic-spectrum conditions and psychotic-spectrum conditions (mainly schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and major depression) represent two major suites of disorders of human cognition, affect, and behavior that involve altered development and function of the social brain. We describe evidence that a large set of phenotypic traits exhibit diametrically opposite phenotypes in autistic-spectrum versus psychotic-spectrum conditions, with a focus on schizophrenia. This suite of traits is inter-correlated, in that autism involves a general pattern of constrained overgrowth, whereas schizophrenia involves undergrowth. These disorders also exhibit diametric patterns for traits related to social brain development, including aspects of gaze, agency, social cognition, local versus global processing, language, and behavior. Social cognition is thus underdeveloped in autistic-spectrum conditions and hyper-developed on the psychotic spectrum.;>We propose and evaluate a novel hypothesis that may help to explain these diametric phenotypes: that the development of these two sets of conditions is mediated in part by alterations of genomic imprinting. Evidence regarding the genetic, physiological, neurological, and psychological underpinnings of psychotic-spectrum conditions supports the hypothesis that the etiologies of these conditions involve biases towards increased relative effects from imprinted genes with maternal expression, which engender a general pattern of undergrowth. By contrast, autistic-spectrum conditions appear to involve increased relative bias towards effects of paternally expressed genes, which mediate overgrowth. This hypothesis provides a simple yet comprehensive theory, grounded in evolutionary biology and genetics, for understanding the causes and phenotypes of autistic-spectrum and psychotic-spectrum conditions.
              Bookmark
              • Record: found
              • Abstract: found
              • Article: found

              Polygenic transmission disequilibrium confirms that common and rare variation act additively to create risk for autism spectrum disorders

              Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) risk is influenced by common polygenic and de novo variation. We aimed to clarify the influence of polygenic risk for ASDs and to identify subgroups of ASD cases, including those with strong acting de novo variants, in which polygenic risk is relevant. Using a novel approach called the polygenic transmission disequilibrium test, and data from 6,454 families with a child with ASD, we show that polygenic risk for ASDs, schizophrenia, and greater educational attainment is over transmitted to children with ASDs. These findings hold independent of proband IQ. We find that polygenic variation contributes additively to risk in ASD cases who carry a strong acting de novo variant. Lastly, we show that elements of polygenic risk are independent and differ in their relationship with phenotype. These results confirm that ASDs’ genetic influences are additive and suggest they create risk through at least partially distinct etiologic pathways.
                Bookmark

                Author and article information

                Contributors
                Journal
                Front Psychiatry
                Front Psychiatry
                Front. Psychiatry
                Frontiers in Psychiatry
                Frontiers Media S.A.
                1664-0640
                24 January 2023
                2023
                : 14
                : 1113592
                Affiliations
                [1] 1Gillberg Neuropsychiatry Centre, Department of Psychiatry and Neurochemistry, Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology, Sahlgrenska Academy, University of Gothenburg , Gothenburg, Sweden
                [2] 2Harvard Medical School , Boston, MA, United States
                [3] 3Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital , Boston, MA, United States
                [4] 4Department of Radiology, Sahlgrenska University Hospital , Gothenburg, Sweden
                [5] 5MedTech West , Gothenburg, Sweden
                Author notes

                Edited by: Daniel Campbell, Michigan State University, United States

                Reviewed by: Leonardo Zoccante, Integrated University Hospital Verona, Italy

                *Correspondence: Darko Sarovic ✉ darko.sarovic@ 123456gu.se

                This article was submitted to Autism, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychiatry

                Article
                10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1113592
                9902494
                4db54a82-91fd-416c-beff-88b4b9592f0b
                Copyright © 2023 Sarovic.

                This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

                History
                : 01 December 2022
                : 09 January 2023
                Page count
                Figures: 0, Tables: 1, Equations: 0, References: 14, Pages: 3, Words: 2019
                Funding
                Funded by: Fredrik och Ingrid Thurings Stiftelse, doi 10.13039/501100003186;
                Funded by: Göteborgs Läkaresällskap, doi 10.13039/501100005689;
                Funded by: Stiftelsen Professor Bror Gadelius Minnesfond, doi 10.13039/501100008586;
                Funded by: Stiftelsen Systrarna Greta Johanssons och Brita Anderssons Minnesfond, doi 10.13039/501100008589;
                Funded by: Sahlgrenska University Hospitals Research Foundations, doi 10.13039/501100014196;
                The author was supported by Fredrik och Ingrid Thurings stiftelse (2020-00581), the Gothenburg Society of Medicine and Kristina Stenborgs Stiftelse (GLS-960453), Stiftelsen Professor Bror Gadelius Minnesfond, Stiftelsen Systrarna Greta Johansson och Brita Anderssons minnesfond, and FoUU Department of Radiology, Sahlgrenska University Hospital.
                Categories
                Psychiatry
                General Commentary

                Clinical Psychology & Psychiatry
                autism,theoretical framework,model,genetic architecture,neurodevelopmental disorder (ndd),rare genetic variant,common genetic variant

                Comments

                Comment on this article