Introduction
Behavioral experiments have shown that anterior thalamic–hippocampal interactions
are critical for spatial and contextual memory (Parker and Gaffan, 1997a,b; Aggleton
and Brown, 1999; Warburton et al., 2000, 2001; Henry et al., 2004). Clinical studies
add further weight to the notion that the anterior thalamic nuclei comprise part of
an extended-hippocampal system that supports key elements of episodic memory (Van
der Werf et al., 2003; Tsivilis et al., 2008; Aggleton et al., 2011; Carlesimo et
al., 2011). The present study sought to understand why the anterior thalamic nuclei
and hippocampus are often interdependent. For this reason, two related experiments
examined the impact of anterior thalamic lesions on the activity of the hippocampus
and related limbic regions by assaying levels of various molecules linked to neuronal
plasticity.
Previous studies have shown how anterior thalamic lesions can reduce activity of the
immediate early gene (IEG) c-fos within the hippocampus (Jenkins et al., 2002a,b;
see also Vann and Albasser, 2009), potentially helping to explain why such lesions
disrupt spatial memory tasks (see Liu et al., 2012). In addition, medial diencephalic
pathology that includes the anterior thalamic nuclei disrupts cholinergic activity
in the hippocampus (Savage et al., 2003; Roland and Savage, 2007). The present study
pursued this inter-relationship by comparing the consequences of lesions in the anterior
thalamic nuclei on activity levels of another IEG, zif268 (also known as egr-1 or
krox24). The IEG zif268 was studied as its expression is often closely associated
with hippocampal plasticity and so it appears to be involved in spatial learning and
memory (Richardson et al., 1992; Abraham et al., 1993, 1994; Herdegen and Leah, 1998;
Tischmeyer and Grimm, 1999; Jones et al., 2001; Guzowski, 2002; Davis et al., 2003;
Lindecke et al., 2006; Kubik et al., 2007; Poirier et al., 2008a). Previous studies
have shown that anterior thalamic lesions lower levels of both c-fos and zif268 in
the retrosplenial cortex (Jenkins et al., 2004b; see also Albasser et al., 2007),
suggesting that anterior thalamic lesions will also cause zif268 hypoactivity in the
hippocampus, i.e., zif268 will again follow the pattern seen with c-fos. Reflecting
this focus on the hippocampus, other closely related sites, e.g., the retrosplenial
and parahippocampal cortices (Diana et al., 2007; Vann et al., 2009) were also examined.
In Experiment 1, all rats received unilateral anterior thalamic lesions to allow inter-hemispheric
comparisons of zif268 activity levels. These rats were divided into two groups (Group
Novel and Group Familiar). One group was given novel objects to explore on the final
test session before IEG analysis while the other group received just familiar objects
to explore. Based on previous measures of c-fos expression using this protocol (Albasser
et al., 2010b) it was expected that the Group Novel rats would show higher perirhinal
and hippocampal IEG expression in the intact hemisphere than Group Familiar, potentially
making it easier to detect any impact of anterior thalamic damage on limbic activation.
The lack of hippocampal zif268 changes after unilateral anterior thalamic damage in
Experiment 1 prompted a second experiment. In Experiment 2, the rats received bilateral
anterior thalamic lesions, so that any null result would not be due to cross-hemispheric
connections. This change meant that a separate surgical control group was required.
The animals were also given a different behavioral task immediately prior to IEG analysis,
the task more explicitly involving spatial learning. Experiment 2 also broadened the
search for hippocampal-related activity changes after anterior thalamic lesions by
looking at three additional molecules, as well as zif268. These additional targets
were: (1) c-AMP response element binding protein (CREB), (2) phosphorylated CREB (pCREB),
and (3) growth associated protein43 (GAP-43). The first two molecular targets were
selected as there is a wealth of evidence highlighting the importance of the conversion
of CREB to pCREB for the consolidation of new learning, including hippocampal-dependent
learning (Silva et al., 1998; Guzowski et al., 2001; Guzowski, 2002; Mizuno et al.,
2002; Winograd and Viola, 2004; Countryman et al., 2005; Warburton et al., 2005).
As part of its functions, pCREB is a transcription factor for the induction of c-fos
and zif268 (Herdegen and Leah, 1998; Silva et al., 1998; Davis et al., 2003). For
these reasons both CREB and pCREB were examined.
The final target, GAP-43, was selected as it is a regulator of growth cone motility
that has been repeatedly associated with neuronal plasticity, e.g., its expression
often changes in neurons undergoing neural growth (Gionotti et al., 1992; Benowitz
and Routtenberg, 1997; Carmichael et al., 2005). Furthermore, GAP-43 is upregulated
following axonal injury and is regarded as a key component of the systems controlling
axonal regeneration (Plunet et al., 2002; Snider et al., 2002). Previous studies have
established that lesions in the anterior thalamic nuclei can have chronic distal effects
on c-fos and zif268 levels in the retrosplenial cortex, yet not cause overt structural
changes in the same region (Jenkins et al., 2004b; Poirier et al., 2008b; Poirier
and Aggleton, 2009). For this reason, GAP-43 is of particular interest as any changes
in this protein might reflect chronic responses to deafferentation that are not revealed
by standard histological methods. Indeed, previous studies have found that hippocampal
changes in GAP-43 can be observed for at least 70 days after entorhinal cortex lesions
(Steward, 1995; Hardman et al., 1997).
Experiment 1
Levels of zif268 expression were analyzed immunohistologically in rats with unilateral
anterior thalamic lesions. The rats were divided into two groups (Novel, Familiar)
according to the nature of their behavioral training. The principal difference between
Group Novel and Group Familiar was whether the rats had explored novel or familiar
objects during the final session prior to immunohistochemical analysis.
Materials and methods
Subjects
The subjects were 25 male Lister Hooded rats (Rattus norvegicus) housed in a 12-h
light/dark cycle and weighing 270–320 g at the beginning of the experiment (Harlan,
Bicester, UK). Water was provided ad libitum throughout, but the rats were maintained
at 85% of their free-feeding weight for the duration of the experiment. The rats were
divided into two groups: Novel (n = 13) and Familiar (n = 12), and where possible
housed in pairs of one Group Novel rat and one Group Familiar rat. Both Experiments
1 and 2 were performed in accordance with the UK Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act
(1986) and associated guidelines, thereby complying with APA ethical standards for
the treatment and care of animals.
Surgery – anterior thalamic lesions
Surgery was performed under pentobarbitone sodium anesthesia (60 mg/kg i.p., Sigma–Aldrich
Company Ltd., Dorset, UK). Once anesthetized, the animal was placed in the head-holder
of the stereotaxic apparatus (Kopf Instruments, CA, USA) with the incisor bar adjusted
to +5.0 relative to the horizontal plane. Following an incision, the scalp was retracted
to expose the scull. A craniotomy was made and the dura cut to expose the cortex above
the target location. Unilateral lesions were made by injecting 0.12 M N-methyl-d-aspartic
acid (NMDA; Sigma Chemicals, UK) dissolved in sterile phosphate buffer (pH 7.4) into
two separate sites within one hemisphere with the use of a 1-μl Hamilton syringe (Hamilton,
Switzerland) that was attached to a moveable arm mounted on the stereotaxic frame.
The other hemisphere was left intact. At each site, 0.22 μl of NMDA was injected over
a period of 5 min, and the syringe was left in situ for an additional 4 min before
being retracted. The anteroposterior (−0.3), mediolateral (±0.9 and ±1.8 from the
midline), and height [dorso-ventral −7.0 (medial site), −6.3 (lateral site)] lesion
coordinates were taken relative to bregma. After the injections, the incision was
cleaned and sutured. A topical antibiotic powder (Aureomycin, Fort Dodge, Animal Health,
Southampton, UK) was applied. The rats received glucose–saline (5 ml s.c.) for fluid
replacement, and were then placed in a recovery chamber until they regained consciousness
(i.e., movement and righting reflex). Rats were then administered an analgesic [paracetamol
mixed with sucrose in the drinking water or Metacam (0.06 ml s.c.; 5 mg/ml meloxicam;
Boehringer Ingelheim Vetmedica, Germany)]. The respiratory stimulant millophyline
(0.1 ml s.c., Arnolds Veterinary Products, Shropshire, UK), the antimicrobial Baytril
(2.5% in their drinking water, Bayer Ltd., Animal Health Division, Ireland), and a
single low dose of diazepam (0.07 ml s.c., 5 mg/ml; CP Pharmaceuticals Ltd., UK) were
administered as required to facilitate post-operative recovery.
Apparatus and materials
The rats were tested in a bow-tie shaped maze (120 cm long, 50 cm wide, and 50 cm
high) made of steel. Each end of the maze formed a triangular arena and these arenas
were joined at their apices by a narrow corridor (12 cm wide). In the center of the
corridor, an opaque guillotine door could be lowered or raised by the experimenter
to allow passage from one end of the bow-tie maze to the other. At the far wall of
each of the triangular arenas were two food wells (3.5 cm in diameter and 2 cm deep)
that were separated by a short, opaque wall that protruded 15 cm from the middle of
the end wall. Objects were placed above these two food wells during the experiment.
The short wall ensured that when exploring one object, the other object could not
be seen. It was, however, easy for the rat to step around the end of the wall.
The study used 147 pairs of objects that differed in size, shape, color, and texture,
but were without any obvious odor to the experimenter. The objects were large enough
to cover the circular food wells but light enough for the rats to displace. The items
were divided into seven groups of 21 pairs of objects.
Behavioral procedures
Pre-training: Behavioral training commenced 4 weeks after surgery. Animals were habituated
for approximately 7 days. By the end of pre-training all rats would run from one end
of the maze to the other and displace objects covering the food wells in order to
obtain a reward (sucrose pellet; 45 mg; Noyes Purified Rodent Diet, Lancaster, NH,
USA). On day 1, pairs of rats were placed in the maze for 20 min and allowed to explore
and consume sucrose pellets scattered along the floor and in the food wells. On day
2, rats were trained individually for 10 min to run back and forth for a reward located
only in the food wells. From day 3, the rats were introduced to the guillotine door
that restricted their movement from one compartment to the other. On day 4, four identical
wood blocks were introduced and gradually covered the food wells, so that by the end
of the 10 min session, the rats would push the blocks in order to obtain the food
reward. From day 5, three other pairs of objects were introduced that varied in size,
shape, color, and weight; with the same two objects (one pair) covering the two wells
in the same side of the apparatus. These three pairs of objects were only used during
pre-training, i.e., they were not used during the experiment proper.
Testing protocols: All rats were tested for 13 sessions, each containing 20 trials.
For Group Novel each session began (Trial 0; see Table 1) with the rat being placed
in one end of the maze that contained two items over the baited food wells, a novel
object (object A1) and a wood block that was familiar from its repeated use in pre-training.
The rat was allowed to explore both objects freely, but after 1 min the guillotine
door was raised allowing access to the second compartment, so starting Trial 1 (Table
1). The rat typically ran immediately to the opposite side of the maze, where again
it could explore two objects that each covered a single sucrose pellet. One of these
objects was novel (object B1) while the other was familiar as it was a duplicate of
object A1 (object A2). After a minute, the guillotine door was raised again and the
rat ran back to the first compartment of the maze (Trial 2) where object C1 (novel)
and a duplicate of familiar object B1 (object B2) were presented. Following a further
minute of free exploration, the guillotine door was raised again (Trial 3), and the
rat ran back into the second compartment to explore a copy of the now familiar object
C1 (object C2) and new object D1 (novel). This sequence continued for a total of 20
trials (Table 1).
All test objects covered one food pellet, which motivated the rats to run back and
forth across the apparatus and approach the objects, but did not affect the validity
of the preferential test of recognition as this relied on differential levels of object
exploration. The rats were placed in a dark, quiet room, for 30 min prior to testing,
and for 60 min at the completion of a session. After 60 min, the rats were returned
to their home cages located in the colony room. All rats initially received 12 sessions,
given over 6 days (one session in the morning and one in the afternoon). For Sessions
1–12, Group Novel was offered a pool of 126 items divided into six sets of 21 object
duplicates. As new sets were used for each of the first six sessions it was necessary
to re-use that same pool of 126 objects for the second six sessions (Sessions 7–12),
though the order and pairings of individual objects changed. Animals were video recorded
throughout training.
The rats in Group Familiar were trained in exactly the same way as Group Novel, except
for one key difference. Group Familiar explored the same set of 21 objects on every
session, i.e., on Sessions 1–12, although the order and individual pairing of objects
changed across sessions. Consequently the rats in this second group should become
very familiar with the individual objects.
On the final test (Session 13) both groups of rats were allowed to explore the same
set of objects, with the order being matched across the two groups. The objects used
were those that had been presented repeatedly to the Group Familiar rats (Sessions
1–12), i.e., all were highly familiar. In contrast, Group Novel had never encountered
these objects before and so all were novel (see Table 1).
Analysis of behavior
Exploration of an object was defined as directing the nose at a distance of <1 cm
to the object and/or touching it with the nose or the paws (including pushing). Sitting
or turning around the object was not included. Likewise, behaviors such as freezing
near the object (at a distance of <1 cm), chewing the object, or carrying the object
in the mouth were not scored as exploration. Sessions 1, 7, and 13 (final) were scored
for their exploration times.
Histology
All rats were perfused 90 min after completion of the final session, the time delay
corresponding to the peak of zif268 protein production after an initiating event (Zangenehpour
and Chaudhuri, 2002). At the end of this delay (which the animals spent in a dark,
quiet room), the animals were injected with an overdose of sodium pentobarbital (1 ml
i.p., 200 mg/ml, Euthatal, Marial Animal Health Ltd., Harlow, Essex, UK) and perfused
intracardially with 0.1 M phosphate buffer saline (PBS) followed by 4% paraformaldehyde
in 0.1 M PBS (PFA). The brains were extracted from the skull and placed on a stirrer
to postfix in PFA for 4 h, after which the brains were placed in 25% sucrose overnight.
The brains were frozen on a microtome (Leica, UK) and sectioned at 40 μm in the coronal
plane. One-in-five sections were mounted and stained with Cresyl Violet, a Nissl stain.
The remaining sections were divided into four (one-in-five sections) series and frozen
in cryoprotectant for later immunohistochemistry.
The size of the lesions in the anterior thalamic nuclei of all 25 rats was estimated
from the Nissl stained tissue. The extent of each lesion was first drawn onto five
equally-spaced, standard sections at different anterior–posterior levels from the
atlas Paxinos and Watson (2005). These drawings were then scanned, and the area of
damage was quantified using the program Analysis^D (Olympus, UK). Next, the percent
damage to the entire anterior thalamus was quantified from these same standard sections.
Zif268 immunohistochemistry and zif-positive cell counts
Sections were removed from the freezer, and washed for 10 min in PBS, four times.
The sections were placed in 10 mM citrate buffer (pH 6) dissolved in deionized water
and incubated in a water bath at 70 °C for 30 min. The sections were then washed in
0.3% hydrogen peroxide in PBS containing 0.2% Triton X-100 (PBST) for 10 min in order
to block endogenous peroxidase activity, and rinsed for 10 min in PBST, four times.
Afterwards, the sections were incubated at 4 °C for 48 h in PBST with rabbit polyclonal
antibody for zif268 (1:3000, C-19, Santa Cruz, Insight Biotechnologies, UK). The sections
were then rinsed for 10 min in PBST, four times. Following the four washes, the sections
were incubated in biotinylated goat anti-rabbit secondary antibody (diluted 1:200
in PBST; Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA, USA) and 1.5% normal goat serum for
2 h. The sections were washed again, and incubated for 1 h in avidin-biotinylated
horseradish peroxidase complex in PBST (Elite Kit, Vector Laboratories). Next, sections
were rinsed in 0.05 M Tris buffer (pH 7.4). The reaction was visualized using diaminobenzidine
(DAB Substrate Kit, Vector Laboratories), and stopped by washing in cold PBS. Finally,
the sections were mounted on gelatine-coated slides, dehydrated through a graded series
of alcohols, and coverslipped.
Sections were viewed on a Leica DMRB microscope, and photographed using an Olympus
DP70 camera. Automated counts of the stained cells were obtained using the program
Analysis^D (Olympus, UK). Cell counts were taken without knowledge of group assignments
and, where possible, without knowledge of lesion hemisphere. Images were gray-scaled,
and the cell detection threshold was set manually. With few exceptions (e.g., a particularly
lightly stained section), the threshold was the same for all sections from the same
processing batch, i.e., held constant between hemispheres and between immunohistochemistry
pairs. Counts of labeled nuclei in each region of interest were determined by counting
those immunopositive cells that were above the detection threshold and between 5–20
μm in size. Counts were made in a frame area of 0.84 × 0.63 mm using 5× magnification.
This frame size enabled all laminae to be included in one image. For larger regions
(e.g., hippocampus), montages of the whole structure of interest were created in the
coronal plane from multiple images. For all brain regions analyzed, between two and
four sections per hemisphere were captured, depending on the size of the region. These
counts were combined to give a mean result.
Regions of interest
The various regions of interest are depicted in Fig. 1. The position and approximate
coordinates of these sites are taken from images by Paxinos and Watson (2005). The
hippocampus was divided into its intermediate (dorsal) and temporal (ventral) parts.
Separate counts were made in CA1, CA3, and the dentate gyrus of the intermediate (dorsal)
hippocampus from around the AP levels −4.80 to −5.64 from bregma (Paxinos and Watson,
2005). Counts were made in temporal (ventral) CA1 and CA3 at a similar level. The
dividing border between the intermediate and temporal hippocampus corresponded to
−5.0 below bregma (Paxinos and Watson, 2005). The most septal portion of the dorsal
hippocampus (anterior to AP −4.80) was, however, avoided as some animals had very
restricted cell loss in the ventral blade of the dentate gyrus at this level. In addition
to these five hippocampal sub-regions, the dorsal subiculum and postsubiculum were
counted as both are directly connected to different parts of the anterior thalamic
nuclei (Meibach and Siegel, 1977; Wright et al., 2010). The lateral entorhinal cortex
(from AP −4.80 to −6.30) was also included as it provides a key link between the perirhinal
cortex and hippocampus (Naber et al., 1999).
Within the parahippocampal region, the perirhinal cortex was examined. The perirhinal
cortex was subdivided into three rostro-caudal subregions (see Albasser et al., 2010b):
rostral (from AP −2.76 to −3.84 relative to bregma), mid (AP −3.84 to −4.80), and
caudal (from AP −4.80 to −6.30). Multiple counts were taken from each of these three
subregions, and their means summed to provide separate totals for areas 35 and 36
(Burwell, 2001). Counts were also taken from the adjacent area TE2 (from AP −4.80
to −6.30), which has also been repeatedly implicated in novelty detection (Wan et
al., 1999; Albasser et al., 2010b; Ho et al., 2011).
The retrosplenial cortex was first subdivided into granular b (Rgb), granular a (Rga),
and dysgranular cortex (Rdg) (van Groen and Wyss, 1990, 1992, 2003). Separate counts
were made for all three sub-regions, with areas Rdg and Rgb being further subdivided
into their rostral (from AP −2.52 to −3.84) and caudal divisions (from AP −4.92 to
−6.24). (This extra subdivision was not made for Rga as it is very restricted anterior
to the splenium.) The retrosplenial cortex was also divided into superficial (layer
II and upper layer III) and deep (lower layers III–VI) as anterior thalamic lesions
can differentially affect these laminae (Jenkins et al., 2002b, 2004b; Poirier et
al., 2008a,b; Poirier and Aggleton, 2009). Finally, the primary auditory cortex was
examined to provide a ‘control’ region, i.e., an area where a priori there was not
expected to be an effect of training condition (Novel versus Familiar) or lesion on
zif268 activity.
Statistics
For the initial analyses, the mean zif268-positive cell counts per area were calculated
for each animal and separated according to hemisphere. It was then possible to compare
these counts in an analysis of variance with one between factor (Group Familiar or
Group Novel) and two within factors [surgery (thalamic lesion or intact hemisphere);
brain site (region of interest)]. The various regions of interest were grouped, e.g.,
hippocampal formation, parahippocampal cortex, and retrosplenial cortex, so that each
analysis of variance (ANOVA) consisted of related areas. The Greenhouse–Geisser correction
was applied when the assumption of sphericity of data was violated. The analysis resulted
in multiple comparisons at the regional level, and so the significance level was further
adjusted using the modified Bonferroni test (Keppel, 1991) to control further for
Type I errors. Therefore, for the hippocampus and retrosplenial cortex (both five
subregions) the significance level was adjusted to 0.04.
Rats were paired (one Group Novel, one Group Familiar) so that the rats in each pair
were trained one after the other, caged together, and perfused one after the other.
This pairing made it possible to normalize data across the two behavioral groups.
This normalization consisted of dividing the mean number of activated neurons in a
given site of the Group Familiar animal by the combined means of the two animals in
each immunohistochemistry pair (Novel and Familiar). The result of this division was
then expressed as a percentage. A score of 50, therefore, corresponded to no change
associated with the recognition task. Because all normalized scores across pairs of
rats sum to 100 it was only necessary to consider the scores from one group (e.g.,
Group Familiar). Separate normalized scores were calculated for the thalamic lesion
hemisphere of each Familiar:Novel pair and the control hemisphere of each Familiar:Novel
pair. The effects of the lesions on task condition (novelty or familiarity) could
then be examined using the normalized scores derived from the intact hemisphere compared
with those from the lesioned hemisphere. As noted above, because of normalization
it was only necessary to analyze the scores from one group, e.g., from Group Familiar,
using a two-way within subjects ANOVA (within factors hemisphere and region). For
direct site to site comparisons the normalized scores cannot be used as the total
score for every site would always be 100.
Results
Unilateral anterior thalamic lesions
Seven animals were excluded from all analyses due to the small size of their lesions
(five from Group Novel and two from Group Familiar). Consequently, Group Novel comprised
eight rats while Group Familiar comprised 10 rats. In these remaining 18 cases, the
cell loss was centered on the anterior thalamic nuclei, which was the sole common
lesion site across all cases. Fig. 2 depicts the lesions in those cases with the smallest
and largest lesions within the two groups. The respective mean tissue loss across
the anterior thalamic nuclei in these rats was 66.7% (Group Familiar) and 67.1% (Group
Novel). The corresponding median scores were 65.6% and 69.3%, respectively. In most
rats there was some sparing in the most caudal parts of these thalamic nuclei as well
as the most medial portion of the anterior medial nucleus. Importantly, although the
NMDA injections created discrete regions of cell loss, these regions did not extend
across the midline into the opposite hemisphere. In some cases (n = 9), there was
damage to the rostral portion of the lateral dorsal nucleus. There was also restricted
cell loss in the medial blade of the septal dentate gyrus immediately dorsal to the
anterior ventral nucleus in twelve of the cases. In one case there was some additional
damage to the fornix. Involvement of the parataenial and reticular nuclei was observed
in only the largest lesions.
Object recognition performance – D1 and D2 discrimination indices
Two indices of object recognition were calculated. Index D1 is the difference in time
spent exploring the novel object and the familiar object. Index D2 then divides this
difference (D1) by the total amount of time spent exploring both objects. Consequently,
the D2 index ranges between +1 and −1. The D2 measure can better compensate for differences
in overall amounts of exploration between animals. The D1 index was calculated by
adding the difference data (D1) from all 20 trials (cumulative D1) while D2 then used
the total exploration data from all trials (cumulative D1/total exploration). These
indices were calculated for Sessions 1, 7, and 13. For Group Novel, indices D1 and
D2 always reflected novelty discrimination, whereas for Group Familiar these same
indices reflected relative recency discriminations for Sessions 7 and 13.
Fig. 3A shows how Group Novel continued to show a marked preference for novel objects
(including Session 13) while the preference shown by Group Familiar diminished over
testing as the individual objects were increasingly re-presented. Reflecting this
pattern, the ANOVA with the between subject factor Group (Novel or Familiar) and the
within factor Session (1, 7, and 13) revealed significant main effects of Group (D1:
F
(1, 16) = 52.2, p < 0.001; D2: F
(1, 16) = 39.0, p < 0.001) and Session (D1: F
(2, 32) = 10.12, p < 0.001; D2: F
(2, 32) = 25.8, p < 0.001), as well as a Group × Session interaction (D1: F
(2, 32) = 8.10, p = 0.001; D2: F
(2, 32) = 8.06, p = 0.001) for both the D1 and D2 scores. Examination of the simple
effects indicated that Group Novel and Group Familiar had comparable D1 and D2 scores
for Session 1 (both p > 0.1). By Session 13 (final session), Group Novel spent significantly
more time than Group Familiar exploring the novel items compared with the familiar
ones (Fig. 3A), reflecting how the objects had become familiar for Group Familiar
(D1: F
(1, 48) = 42.4, p < 0.001; D2: F
(1, 48) = 25.0, p < 0.001).
One-sample t-tests confirmed that both groups explored the novel objects significantly
more than the familiar objects during Session 1 (D1 for Group Novel: t
7 = 8.06, p < 0.001; D1 for Group Familiar: t
9 = 7.66, p < 0.001; D2 for Group Novel t
7 = 16.6, p < 0.001; D2 for Group Familiar: t
9 = 11.4, p < 0.001), i.e., the rats recognized the novel objects. By Session 13 (final)
Group Novel still spent considerably more time exploring the novel items than the
familiar ones (D1: t
7 = 8.33, p < 0.001; D2: t
7 = 14.1, p < 0.001). Group Familiar also still showed a recency discrimination (one-sample
t test, D1: t
9 = 3.18, p = 0.011; D2: t
9 = 2.25, p = 0.051), although the level of discrimination was appreciably lower than
that in Group Novel.
Cumulative total exploration
Although the two groups initially showed comparable total levels of object exploration
(Session 1, Fig. 3B), a difference emerged by the final session, reflecting the increased
exploration of the novel objects (Fig. 3B). An ANOVA examining these cumulative exploration
levels yielded a significant main effect of Group (F
(1, 16) = 5.38, p = 0.034) and Session (F
(2, 32) = 4.73, p = 0.016). Although the Group × Session interaction was not significant
(p > 0.1), the simple effects revealed that on the final session Group Familiar explored
the objects significantly less compared with Group Novel (F
(1, 48) = 5.98, p = 0.018).
Immediate-early gene (zif268) results
The zif268 analyses involved eight Group Novel and ten Group Familiar rats. All 18
rats were included in those comparisons based on raw cell counts (between subjects).
For the normalized counts (within subjects) the principal data came from pairs that
had been grouped throughout training and were subsequently reacted together. This
behavioral pairing was not possible in every case as those lesions that were unacceptable
could only be defined post histology. Consequently, one rat from Group Familiar was
added to a Familiar:Novel pairing, and all three rats immunohistochemically reacted
together (i.e., as a triplicate). There were two such triplicate groups (two Group
Familiar, one Group Novel), alongside the six standard pairings (one Group Familiar,
one Group Novel). The data from the triplicate groups were transformed to match the
overall totals for the standard pairings (i.e., for the triplicates when normalization
took place, each of the two rats from Group Familiar were normalized to the same rat
from Group Novel, but as the rat from Group Novel would then have two normalized scores,
the mean of these two scores was used).
Hippocampal subfields
The initial analyses compared the raw scores of zif268-positive cells in five hippocampal
sub-regions and compared the counts from the intact and lesioned hemispheres. Despite
marked differences in the absolute zif268 counts from region to region (F
(4, 60) = 114.1, p < 0.001), there were no systematic differences between the lesioned
and intact hemispheres (p > 0.1). The overall analysis of variance (one between, two
within factors) also found no evidence of a task effect (Novel versus Familiar, p > 0.1),
and no three-way interaction between region, lesion, and task condition (p > 0.1).
The next analyses used the data normalized according to their Familiar:Novel pairings
(Fig. 4A). Differential behavioral effects (Novel versus Familiar) will cause the
scores to deviate from chance such that a score significantly below 50 indicates that
Group Familiar had lower zif268 counts than Group Novel. An ANOVA showed that these
normalized scores differed across the regions of interest (F
(4, 32) = 3.23, p = 0.025), i.e., that some sub-regions had significantly different
reactions to the Familiar:Novel manipulation. In particular, temporal CA1 and temporal
CA3 showed differential responses, with relatively increased zif268 scores with novel
objects in CA1, but the opposite pattern in CA3. There was, however, no task by lesion
interaction (p > 0.1), i.e., this differential response to Novel versus Familiar objects
was not modulated by the anterior thalamic lesion. One-sample t-tests showed that
none of regions (CA1, CA3) in either the sham or lesion hemisphere differed significantly
from the chance score of 50 (for all analyses, p > 0.1).
Perirhinal cortex and area TE2
Although there were large differences in raw zif268 counts (F
(2, 30) = 68.5, p < 0.001) across the three sub-regions (TE2 and perirhinal areas
35 and 36), there was no overall effect of lesion (p > 0.1) or behavioral condition
(Novel versus Familiar, p > 0.1), and no significant interactions between these manipulations
(all, p > 0.1). Next, the normalized scores were examined to look at the impact of
the test condition and lesion status (as above, scores lower than 50 represent a lowering
of the zif268 counts in Group Familiar compared with Group Novel; Fig. 4B). In the
Sham hemisphere there appeared to be a relative reduction of zif268 in area TE2 and,
to a lesser degree, in parts of the perirhinal region associated with exploring familiar
objects. One-sample t-tests revealed that only the area TE2 scores appeared to be
below chance in the control hemisphere, but this effect was not quite significant
(t
8 = 2.22, p = 0.057). These parahippocampal changes yielded a borderline overall effect
of lesion (F
(1, 8) = 4.69, p = 0.062) but no clear site by lesion interaction (F
(2, 16) = 3.51, p = 0.091; Greenhouse–Geisser correction). However, an examination
of the simple effects indicated that in area TE2 there was a greater reduction of
zif268 activity in Group Familiar in the intact hemisphere compared with the lesioned
hemisphere (F
(1, 24) = 11.5, p = 0.002). It was possible to divide each of the two main perirhinal
regions (areas 35 and 36) into a rostral, mid, and caudal sub-division, but this fine
grained analysis did not reveal any significant changes (all p > 0.1).
Subicular and entorhinal cortices
Comparisons across the three target regions (dorsal subiculum, postsubiculum, and
lateral entorhinal cortex) found raw zif268 count differences (F
(2, 30) = 53.0, p < 0.001), along with evidence that the thalamic lesions reduced
some IEG counts (F
(1, 15) = 4.01, p = 0.064). Simple effects indicated that the thalamic lesions reduced
zif268 counts in the postsubiculum (F
(1, 45) = 7.16, p = 0.010), although there was no site by lesion interaction (p > 0.1).
Finally, analysis of the normalized scores failed to show that the behavioral task
affected zif268 levels in specific sites (p > 0.1) or that this measure was differentially
affected by the surgery (p > 0.1; Fig. 4C).
Retrosplenial cortex
Consistent with previous studies, anterior thalamic lesions reduced zif268 activity
across much of the retrosplenial cortex. Consequently, analyses of the raw scores
revealed an effect of lesion (F
(1, 16) = 7.94, p = 0.012) as well as of site (F
(4, 64) = 56.9, p < 0.001). There was, however, no overall effect of behavioral condition
(p > 0.1) or any significant interactions between the above (for all p > 0.1). Simple
effects indicated that thalamic lesions significantly reduced zif268 counts in both
rostral Rgb (F
(1, 80) = 6.38, p = 0.014) and caudal Rgb (F
(1, 80) = 6.99, p = 0.010), but there were no significant differences elsewhere (p > 0.1
for all). These lesion effects in Rgb were further explored by comparing the counts
in the superficial and deep layers. The results indicated that for both rostral and
caudal Rgb, the reduction of zif268 activity caused by the lesion was in both the
superficial layers (rostral: F
(1, 64) = 6.94, p = 0.011; caudal: F
(1, 64) = 6.05, p = 0.017) and the deep layers (rostral: F
(1, 64) = 11.2, p = 0.001; caudal: F
(1, 64) = 14.4, p < 0.001).
To examine any differential effects of the behavioral conditions, the normalized data
were considered (Fig. 4D). First, there was no clear evidence that the behavioral
task (Novel versus Familiar) differentially affected the various retrosplenial sub-regions
(overall region effect, p > 0.1), and there was no interaction with lesion condition
(p > 0.1). Inspection of the data, however, indicated that caudal dysgranular retrosplenial
cortex (cRdg) had higher zif268 counts for the novel condition in the sham hemisphere
(t
9 = 2.30, p = 0.047) with a similar, but nonsignificant, increase in zif268 counts
in the lesion hemisphere (t
9 = 2.02, p = 0.074).
Control cortex (primary auditory cortex)
As expected, inspection of the raw counts in the auditory cortex did not yield a significant
effect of recognition group, lesion, or a group × lesion interaction (p > 0.1 for
all). Similarly, there was no indication that the anterior thalamic lesions modulated
task performance (p > 0.1), or that the recognition condition influenced normalized
zif268 counts (i.e., the counts did not differ from chance for either the intact or
lesion hemisphere; both p > 0.1).
Experiment 2
The impact of bilateral anterior thalamic lesions on the activity of zif268, CREB,
pCREB, and GAP-43 was examined in the same core limbic structures as Experiment 1.
In addition, counts were made in two frontal brain areas, the prelimbic cortex (PL;
around AP +2.76) and infralimbic cortex (IL; around AP +2.76). The choice of a spatial
task prior to perfusion led to the selection of a slightly different set of parahippocampal
regions, targeting regions such as the medial entorhinal cortex (mEnt) as it had previously
been shown to increase IEG activity following spatial learning in this same apparatus
(Wan et al., 1999; Vann et al., 2000; Aggleton et al., 2012). The control rats and
those with anterior thalamic lesions were trained on a series of behavioral tasks
prior to the present study (Aggleton et al., 2009). These behavioral studies showed
that the anterior thalamic lesions were sufficient to impair T-maze alternation and
to disrupt learning the geometric properties of a rectangular watermaze (discriminating
long from short walls to identify a location). These deficits thus confirmed the effectiveness
of the lesions. The same rats did, however, successfully learn a series of complex,
configural visual discriminations (‘structural learning’ tasks) that were also trained
in a watermaze (Aggleton et al., 2009).
Prior to perfusion, the animals received a behavioral task with the purpose of altering
levels of pCREB and zif268. For this purpose, rats were exposed to a novel room while
running up and down the arms of a radial-maze. Anterior thalamic lesions (Aggleton
et al., 1996; Byatt and Dalrymple-Alford, 1996) impair working memory tasks in the
radial-maze, leading to abnormal patterns of arm choice. For this reason, the experimenter
rather than the rat selected the choice of arms, so matching behavioral performance
across the two groups. This radial maze task took place approximately 6 months after
surgery.
Materials and methods
Subjects
Thirteen male rats (R. norvegicus) of the pigmented DA (Dark Agouti) strain (Harlan,
Bicester, UK) were used in Experiment 2. All rats were housed in pairs (the additional
fourteenth animal was not part of the study) under diurnal conditions, water was provided
ad libitum throughout. The rats were maintained at 85% of their free-feeding weight
for the duration of the experiment. At the time of surgery the animals were aged 4 months
and weighed 220–250 g.
Surgery – anterior thalamic lesions
Seven naïve rats received anterior thalamic lesions (ATN2, n = 7). The procedure was
as Experiment 1, with minor changes reflecting the smaller size of the animals. The
thalamic lesions were made by injecting 0.20 μl of 0.12 NMDA (Sigma Chemicals, Poole,
UK) dissolved in PBS at pH 7.2 into two sites per hemisphere using a 1-μl syringe
(Hamilton, Switzerland). The stereotaxic coordinates were as follows: anterior–posterior,
−0.5 from bregma; medio-lateral, 1.0 and 1.7 from the midline; dorso-ventral, −6.3
and −5.7 from the top of the dura for the medial and lateral injections, respectively.
For the six surgical control rats (Sham2) a surgical needle was lowered twice per
hemisphere through the cortex above the anterior thalamic nuclei, but not into subcortical
regions.
Apparatus and materials
Testing occurred in an eight-arm radial-maze consisting of an octagonal central platform
(34-cm diameter) and eight equally spaced radial arms (87 cm long, 10 cm wide). The
floors of the central platform and the arms were made of wood, while panels of clear
Perspex (24-cm high) formed the walls of the arms. At the end of each arm was a food
well (2 cm in diameter and 0.5 cm deep). At the base of each arm was a clear Perspex
guillotine door (12 cm high) that controlled access in and out of the central octagonal
area. The maze was on a stand 63 cm high that could be revolved. Each door was attached
to a pulley system enabling the experimenter to open and close access to each arm.
The maze was placed in a rectangular room (255 cm × 330 cm × 260 cm), lit by two banks
of fluorescent strip lights (0.5 m long, luminance 1022 lux) positioned over the center
of the maze.
Behavioral procedures
Rats were trained to run down pre-selected arms of an eight-arm radial-maze to retrieve
sucrose reward pellets (45 mg; Noyes Purified Rodent Diet, Lancaster, NH, USA) from
the end of the arm. At the beginning of each block of eight trials, all arms were
baited with a single sucrose pellet. The experimenter controlled access to each arm
by using a pulley system to open the guillotine door at the start of the arm (a trial
was completed once all eight arms had been visited). After all arms had been visited,
the rat was contained in the central compartment of the maze for approximately 2 min
while all arms were re-baited. Each animal ran one session per day over a 4 day period.
Each session lasted 20 min, and consisted of multiple trials in the radial-arm maze,
one after the other. Different randomized arm sequences were used throughout.
On the final test day (Session 4) the animals performed the same task as above but
in an identical radial maze in a novel room. Although all animals were forced to visit
the same arms in the same order, because of variations in the time it took individual
rats to complete a trial, a 20-min time limit was set for each session. Each animal
was placed in a holding-box in a dark, quiet room for 30 min before and 90 min after
each radial-arm maze session. The holding box measured 10 cm × 10 cm × 26 cm and was
made of aluminum throughout so that the lid, floor, and all walls were opaque.
Histology
The procedures matched those for Experiment 1, unless otherwise stated. Ninety minutes
after completing the final radial-arm maze session, rats were deeply anesthetized
with pentobarbitone sodium (140 mg/kg) and transcardially perfused with 0.1 M phosphate-buffer
(PB) followed by 4% paraformaldehyde in 0.1 M PB containing 50 mM of sodium fluoride.
The brains were rapidly removed and postfixed for 4 h in 4% paraformaldehyde, before
being transferred to 30% sucrose overnight. Coronal sections were then cut at 40 μm
on a freezing microtome and collected in separate dishes to undergo staining with
various antibodies. Two series were collected in 0.1 M PBST for staining with zif268
and GAP-43, and two series (one in five) were collected in 0.1 M PB containing 5%
sucrose and 50 mM sodium fluoride for staining with anti-CREB and anti-pCREB. The
containers with the sections for CREB and pCREB were covered in foil. A separate one-in-five
series of sections was mounted directly onto gelatine-coated slides and stained using
Cresyl Violet, a Nissl stain, for histological identification of specific brain regions.
Immunohistochemistry and cell counts
zif268 and GAP-43: The procedures were essentially identical to those described for
zif268 in Experiment 1 although the tissue was not placed in 10 mM citrate buffer
for 30 min as this buffer can degrade the sections. The only other difference was
that for GAP-43, sections were incubated in PBST containing GAP-43 rabbit polyclonal
antibody [1:3000; (H-100) Santa Cruz Biotechnology, USA].
CREB and pCREB: Sections were incubated for 1 h in 0.1 M phosphate buffer pH 7.4 (PB)
blocking serum containing 50 mM NaF, 0.3% Triton X-100 and 3% bovine serum albumin
(BSA). The sections were covered at all times. Sections were next incubated in anti-CREB
and anti-pCREB rabbit polyclonal antibodies (1:2000; Upstate Biotechnology, USA) diluted
in Trizma base solution containing 0.3% Triton X-100 (TTBS) with 50 mM NaF and 3%
BSA, overnight at room temperature with gentle shaking. Again sections were kept covered.
Sections were then washed several times in PB and incubated in biotinylated goat anti-rabbit
secondary antibody (diluted 1:200 in TTBS; Vectastain, Vector Laboratories, Burlingame,
CA, USA) and 1.5% normal goat serum for 2 h at room temperature on a rotator. Sections
were then washed in PB and processed with avidin-biotinylated horseradish peroxidase
complex in TTBS (Elite Kit, Vector Laboratories) for 1 h at room temperature, again
with constant rotation, and then covered. Sections were washed again in PB and then
the reaction was visualized using diaminobenzidine (DAB Substrate Kit, Vector Laboratories).
The reaction was stopped by washing in cold PBS, and then the sections were mounted
on gelatine-coated slides and cover-slipped.
Cell counts and statistics: Wherever possible, the experimenter was blind to the group
identity of each animal. In addition, all counts used a standardized procedure so
that the threshold was set automatically, based on the overall intensity of the image.
For each brain area, counts of immunopositive cells were taken from at least three
sections from each hemisphere and the six or more counts then averaged to produce
a mean score for each animal. The analyses matched those for Experiment 1 except there
were no additional factors relating to behavioral protocols. All rats were paired
immunohistochemically and analyzed as pairs using their cell counts data (as in Experiment
1). Pair-data were not, however, then normalized (unlike Experiment 1) as all rats
received the same behavioral experiences, i.e., there was not the additional factor
of training condition. Counts from the retrosplenial cortex were again subdivided
between superficial (I to upper III) and deep (mid III–VI) layers, in response to
previous findings that changes in c-fos activity after anterior thalamic lesions are
most pronounced in the superficial layers of the granular retrosplenial cortex (Jenkins
et al., 2004b; Poirier et al., 2008a,b; Poirier and Aggleton, 2009). In addition,
separate analyses were conducted for just the granular retrosplenial cortex following
the finding from Experiment 1 that zif268 changes were most apparent in this subdivision
of the cortical area.
Results
Bilateral anterior thalamic lesions
Of the seven rats with anterior thalamic lesions, one was excluded as the thalamic
lesions were unusually small. The anterior thalamic lesions in the remaining six rats
consistently produced appreciable degrees of cell loss in the anterior dorsal and
anterior ventral thalamic nuclei (Fig. 5). In every case the anterior thalamic nuclei
were clearly shrunken in all three planes as a result of the cell loss. The respective
mean tissue loss across the anterior thalamic nuclei in these rats was 39.2%, with
a median score of 42% (range 23.2–61.7%). The anterior dorsal nucleus was the most
consistently atrophied, while the anterior medial nucleus showed the greatest degree
of sparing (Fig. 5). Other rostral thalamic nuclei were left intact, although unilateral
damage to the dorsal part of the rostral portion of the lateral dorsal nucleus was
seen in five cases, with bilateral damage in the same region in one further case.
Some very restricted cell loss was sometimes present at the rostral limit of the very
medial margin of the septal hippocampus. This very restricted cell loss, which included
parts of the medial blade of the dentate gyrus, was unilateral in three cases and
bilateral in three cases. The final group sizes were; ATN2 n = 6, Sham2 n = 6.
Radial-arm maze behavior
Although all animals were forced to visit the same arms in the same order, there were
individual differences in the total numbers of arms visited within the 20-min time
period. The two groups did not differ on the total numbers of arms visited [mean ATN2 = 16.0
(sd 6.9), Sham2 = 14.2 (sd 4.3), p > 0.1].
Immunohistochemical (zif268, CREB, PCREB, GAP-43) results
For the various regions within each grouping there were often significant differences
in the raw cell counts for a given marker. These changes are only reported if they
appear to be affected by the thalamic lesion. Because of the close relationship between
CREB and pCREB (Fig. 6), we also compared their scores in combined analyses for those
sites where there was evidence of a lesion effect for one or both of the molecules.
Hippocampal subfields
zif268: The first analyses compared the separate cell counts taken in the five subfields
of the hippocampus; dorsal (intermediate) CA1, dorsal (intermediate) CA3, dorsal (intermediate)
dentate gyrus, ventral (temporal) CA1 and ventral (temporal) CA3. There was no group
difference (p > 0.1) and no group by subfield interaction (p > 0.1; Fig. 7A).
GAP-43: There was no evidence of a difference in GAP-43 counts between the two group
(p > 0.1), neither was there a group by region interaction (p > 0.1; Fig. 7B).
CREB: There was no evidence that the rats with anterior thalamic lesions showed a
consistent change in CREB levels across the hippocampus (p > 0.1) and there was no
group by area interaction (p > 0.1; Figs. 6, 7C).
pCREB: Overall, there appeared to be a reduction in pCREB cell-positive counts in
the rats with anterior thalamic lesions (Figs. 6, 7D), and this change was significant
(F
(1, 10) = 9.04, p = 0.013). No group by area interaction was observed (p > 0.1). The
simple effects indicated that pCREB levels were reduced in the intermediate dentate
gyrus (F
(1, 50) = 8.18, p = 0.006).
pCREB versus CREB: An analysis of variance (two within and one between factor) helped
to confirm the different impact of anterior thalamic damage upon these two molecules.
There was a significant interaction between the levels of these two proteins in the
hippocampus as only pCREB decreased after anterior thalamic lesions (F
(1, 10) = 7.09, p = 0.024). The three-way interaction (F
(4, 40) = 2.66, p = 0.047) reflected the finding that this differential change was
larger for some regions (CA1, dentate gyrus) than others (CA3).
Subicular and entorhinal cortices
zif268: The first analyses compared the separate cell counts taken in the postsubiculum,
dorsal subiculum, and medial entorhinal cortex. Overall the ATN group had lower zif268
cell-positive counts (F
(1, 10) = 7.18, p = 0.023), but this effect was region specific as shown by the group
by subfield interaction (F
(2, 20) = 8.56, p = 0.002). Simple effects showed that the significant impact of the
thalamic lesions was confined to the postsubiculum, where the lesions produced a large
drop in zif268 positive cells (F
(1, 30) = 23.73, p < 0.001; Fig. 8A).
GAP-43: There was no evidence of a thalamic lesion effect (p > 0.1) and no lesion
by region interaction (p > 0.1; Fig. 8B).
CREB: Likewise, there was no evidence of a lesion group difference in the numbers
of CREB-positive cells (p > 0.1) nor any group by area interaction (p > 0.1; Fig.
8C).
pCREB: Again, no evidence was found that the lesions affected overall cell-positive
counts across the three areas (p > 0.1), and no group by area interaction was observed
(p > 0.1; Fig. 8D).
Retrosplenial cortex
Counts were first made in the same five subfields as in Experiment 1. Additional analyses
focused on the granular retrosplenial cortex as this subdivision showed consistent
zif268 changes in Experiment 1 following anterior thalamic damage.
zif268: Overall, the ATN group had lower zif268 cell-positive counts (F
(1, 10) = 8.35, p = 0.016) but this effect was selective as shown by the group by
subfield interaction (F
(4, 40) = 22.4, p < 0.001; Fig. 9A). Simple effects showed that the thalamic lesions
produced a significant drop in zif268 positive cells in caudal granular retrosplenial
cortex, area a (cRga), caudal granular retrosplenial cortex, area b (cRgb), cRdg (all
p < 0.01), i.e., in caudal parts of the retrosplenial cortex. Analyses of the three
granular subfields revealed a significant interaction between layer (superficial or
deep) and surgical condition (F
(1, 10) = 8.63, p = 0.015) reflecting the greater impact of the thalamic surgeries
on the more superficial granular retrosplenial cortex.
GAP-43: There was no evidence of a group difference in the numbers of GAP-43 positive
cells (p > 0.1) or any group by subfield interaction (p > 0.1; Fig. 9B). Likewise,
there was no evidence of a difference between the sensitivity of superficial and deep
cortical layers (p > 0.1).
CREB: There were no significant lesion related changes in CREB positive cells across
the five subfields that comprised the area (F
(1, 10) = 3.16, p = 0.106) and no group by subfield interaction (p > 0.1; Fig. 9C).
Additional CREB analyses focused on just the granular (rostral Rgb, caudal Rgb, Rga)
retrosplenial cortex for both superficial and deep layers. While mean CREB levels
appeared reduced following anterior thalamic lesions, this change was not significant
(p > 0.1) and there was no interaction with region or layers (superficial or deep;
p > 0.1 for both).
pCREB: Although there were signs of a reduction in pCREB-positive cells across the
region, this effect was not significant (F
(1, 10) = 4.24, p = 0.067) and there was no group by area interaction (p > 0.1; Fig.
9D). Inspection of the simple effects indicated that ATN lesions significantly reduced
the pCREB counts in caudal Rgb (F
(1, 50) = 5.70, p = 0.021).
Again, in view of the consistent zif268 changes in granular retrosplenial cortex in
both Experiments 1 and 2, additional pCREB analyses examined just the granular (rostral
Rgb, caudal Rgb, Rga) cortex for both superficial and deep retrosplenial layers. A
significant reduction in pCREB was found following anterior thalamic lesions (F
(1, 10) = 7.15 p = 0.023), though there was no interaction with region (p > 0.1) as
the reduction was not disproportionate in any single area. There were also significantly
more pCREB counts in the deep compared with superficial layers (F
(1, 10) = 9.64, p = 0.011), but there was no interaction with either region or group
(p > 0.1). Simple effects revealed a significant fall in pCREB in the superficial
part of caudal Rgb (F
(1, 60) = 4.37, p = 0.041). When the pCREB and CREB data were analyzed together there
was no evidence of an interaction (p > 0.1) as levels of the two markers did not diverge.
Prefrontal cortex
zif268: Overall, the ATN group appeared to have higher zif268-positive cell counts
although this change was not significant (p > 0.1; Fig. 10A).
GAP-43: While there was no overall change in GAP-43-positive cells between the two
groups (p > 0.1), there was a group by area interaction (F
(1, 10) = 7.81; p = 0.019). This interaction reflected the slight increase in counts
for the Sham group going from IL to PL (F
(1, 10) = 5.05, p = 0.049), a pattern not seen in the ATN2 group (Fig. 10B).
CREB: Although the mean counts of CREB-positive cells appeared lower in the ATN group,
this change was not significant (p > 0.1) and there was no interaction (p > 0.1; Fig.
10C).
pCREB: There was no evidence of a change in the numbers of pCREB-positive cells across
the two areas (p > 0.1; Fig. 10D).
Control cortex (primary auditory cortex)
Analyses based on the raw cell counts found no evidence of a difference between the
Sham2 and ATN2 groups for either zif268, GAP-43, CREB, or pCREB (all p ⩾ 0.1).
Discussion
Lesions were placed in the anterior thalamic nuclei of rats in two experiments. Measurements
were then made of several molecules linked to neuronal plasticity in a set of limbic
sites related to anterior thalamic function. In Experiment 1, rats were exposed to
either novel or familiar objects prior to analysis of the immediate-early gene zif268,
and so the experiment included sites presumed to be involved in object recognition
memory. In Experiment 2, all rats were exposed to novel room cues in a radial-arm
maze, and so the study included sites thought to be involved in spatial learning.
Experiment 2 examined CREB, pCREB, and GAP-43, in addition to zif268. Given the various
differences between Experiments 1 and 2, any changes in zif268 that were common to
both experiments are presumably highly reliable. In fact, the most reliable changes
were in the granular retrosplenial cortex (area 29) where anterior thalamic lesions
consistently reduced zif268 activity, irrespective of whether the lesion was unilateral
or bilateral, the strain of rat, or the behavioral task that the rat had performed
immediately prior to IEG measurement (see also Jenkins et al., 2004b; Poirier and
Aggleton, 2009). In addition to the retrosplenial cortex, the postsubiculum also showed
reduced zif268 activity following anterior thalamic lesions in both experiments. No
consistent zif268 changes were found in any of the other sites studied, despite the
fact that most of the sites examined have direct connections with the anterior thalamic
nuclei (Shibata 1992, 1993a,b).
A central feature of the present study was the analysis of rats with unilateral thalamic
lesions in Experiment 1 and bilateral anterior thalamic lesions in Experiment 2. This
design was selected because of inherent shortcomings with either approach. Unilateral
lesions (Experiment 1) have the advantage that all comparisons can be made across
hemispheres within the same animal, i.e., they are exceptionally well controlled for
perceptual-motor factors, but there is the risk that null results might reflect fibers
that cross between the hemispheres. Bilateral anterior thalamic lesions help minimize
the impact of any crossing fibers but, unlike unilateral lesions, they are sufficient
to induce marked learning changes, e.g., to spatial learning (Warburton et al., 2001;
Aggleton et al., 2009). As a consequence, there might be chronic changes in the animals’
behavior that could lead to alterations in the status of limbic structures. Consequently,
there remains the possibility of changes to structures, such as the hippocampus, that
are secondary, rather than primary. It is this possibility that should be excluded
by investigating rats with unilateral thalamic lesions.
An integral part of Experiment 1 was the contrast between rats that had just explored
novel objects and rats that had just explored familiar objects. Advantages associated
with the bow-tie maze protocol include the ability to give each rat multiple trials
within a session, yet without the need to handle the rat between trials (Albasser
et al., 2010a). One potential disadvantage was that the overall times spent exploring
objects could not be fully matched on the final session between Group Novel and Group
Familiar. This time difference arose as an almost inevitable consequence of repeating
the same objects across all previous sessions for Group Familiar, i.e., the resultant
decreases in spontaneous exploration confirmed that the rats in this group correctly
perceived the repeated objects as familiar. In order to match total exploration times
across the two treatment groups it would have been necessary to give Group Familiar
extra trials with additional objects, but this would introduce other differences.
With these considerations in mind, it can be noted that the baiting procedure should
help to maintain comparable patterns of behavior across the two groups, i.e., rats
approached and manipulated all objects.
A previous study using the same behavioral design as Experiment 1 found that novel
stimuli raised c-fos activity in caudal perirhinal cortex, area TE2, and hippocampal
subfields CA3 and CA1, while the dentate gyrus decreased its c-fos activity (Albasser
et al., 2010b). The present study extended this protocol to measure zif268 responses.
While some differential hippocampal zif268 activity was associated with the novel
versus familiar stimuli, this effect appeared localized. In particular, temporal CA1
showed a relative increase in zif268 activity associated with novel objects compared
with temporal CA3, which showed a decrease. These changes contrasted with a lack of
any differential zif268 changes in the perirhinal cortex, despite the known importance
of this cortical area for object recognition whether tested in an open arena or in
a bow-tie maze (Ennaceur et al., 1996; Winters et al., 2008; Albasser et al., 2010a).
In fact, the current null result for zif268 in the perirhinal cortex is not unexpected
as earlier studies had found c-fos, but not zif268, activity changes in the perirhinal
cortex when rats are shown novel visual stimuli (Brown and Xiang, 1998; Wan et al.,
1999; Aggleton et al., 2012; see also Romero-Grandados et al., 2010). The implication
is that zif268 activity in the perirhinal cortex is not a key process for effective
long-term object recognition memory (although see Jones et al., 2001; Bozon et al.,
2003). In contrast, the functional significance of the perirhinal c-fos response for
object recognition memory has recently been demonstrated by blocking c-fos activity
in this cortical area, and finding that this manipulation impairs recognition memory
after extended retention delays (Seoane et al., 2012). There remains a concern that
the relatively localized changes in zif268 activity associated with the behavioral
tasks in Experiment 1 may have reduced the sensitivity of the protocol for looking
at lesion effects. While this concern cannot be dismissed, it should be noted that
the raw zif268 counts in all hippocampal areas examined remained high and that local
differential hippocampal responses (e.g., in temporal CA1) were identified. Consequently,
there remained the potential for seeing lesion-induced changes in hippocampal zif268,
but they were not observed.
As noted, some hippocampal subfields show differential c-fos (Albasser et al., 2010b)
and zif268 (present study) responses to novel versus familiar stimuli when tested
in the bow-tie maze. These findings might suggest that the rodent hippocampus has
a direct role in supporting object recognition memory. This conclusion should, however,
be treated with caution. The first reason is that extensive hippocampal lesions do
not appear to alter object recognition memory when it is tested in the bow-tie maze,
even when using a range of retention intervals (Albasser et al., 2010a, 2012). A second
reason is that on encountering a novel object, rats will spontaneously learn much
about its associative features, e.g., its spatial and temporal location. This spontaneous
associative learning is hippocampal dependent (Save et al., 1992; Ennaceur et al.,
1997; Mumby et al., 2002; Piterkin et al., 2008; Warburton and Brown, 2010; Barker
and Warburton, 2011; Albasser et al., 2012) and is consistently associated with changes
in hippocampal c-fos activity (Wan et al., 1999; Jenkins et al., 2004a; Amin et al.,
2006). Given these results, the local changes in hippocampal zif268 found in this
behavioral task are not unexpected and need not directly reflect object recognition.
The major finding from Experiment 2 concerned the relative changes in CREB and pCREB
levels in the hippocampus following anterior thalamic damage. While hippocampal pCREB
levels were significantly lower in those rats with anterior thalamic lesions compared
to the Sham2 rats, hippocampal CREB levels did not show this pattern. Consequently
there was a significant interaction as CREB and pCREB showed different relative profiles.
These findings are notable because the conversion of CREB to pCREB within the hippocampus
is seen as a key step for consolidating spatial learning (Guzowski and McGaugh, 1997;
Guzowski, 2002; Mizuno et al., 2002; Winograd and Viola, 2004). Consequently, the
profile of results in the ATN2 rats suggests a failure of this conversion mechanism
within the hippocampus, at least when rats are placed in the radial-arm maze. This
finding is potentially revealing as it is already known that CREB phosphorylation
in the hippocampus correlates with learning in the radial-arm maze (Mizuno et al.,
2002), and that both hippocampal lesions and anterior thalamic lesions impair spatial
learning in this same apparatus (Olton et al., 1979; Aggleton et al., 1996; Warburton
et al., 2001). Furthermore, cross-lesion disconnections have shown that the hippocampus
and anterior thalamic nuclei have interdependent roles in supporting spatial learning,
including radial-arm maze learning (Warburton et al., 2001; see also Olton et al.,
1982; Henry et al., 2004). It would, therefore, appear that CREB phosphorylation in
the hippocampus is under the partial control of the anterior thalamic nuclei. Such
a mechanism might be expected to affect principally the long-term maintenance of spatial
information (Guzowski and McGaugh, 1997).
To appreciate the wider implications of this finding is it useful to note that the
conversion of CREB to pCREB is a key step in learning-induced plasticity that includes
the regulation of various IEGs, including c-fos and zif268 (Guzowski and McGaugh,
1997; Silva et al., 1998; Guzowski, 2002; Winograd and Viola, 2004; Countryman et
al., 2005). Consistent with this mechanism, previous studies have found that anterior
thalamic lesions reliably reduce hippocampal c-fos activity (Jenkins et al., 2002a,b).
This c-fos hypoactivity could, therefore, reflect changes in CREB phosphorylation
associated with anterior thalamic damage, although it must be selective in that zif268
levels are not equally affected. Further support for the notion that hippocampal IEG
disruption (e.g., of c-fos) contributes to the functional deficits after anterior
thalamic lesions comes from the finding that normal rats placed in a radial-arm maze
surrounded by novel spatial cues (as in the present study) show increased c-fos activity
both in the hippocampus and anterior thalamus (Vann et al., 2000; Jenkins et al.,
2002b). In addition, blockade of hippocampal c-fos activity disrupts radial-arm maze
performance (He et al., 2002) and the re-activation of hippocampal c-fos positive
cells can seemingly resurrect spatial memories (Liu et al., 2012). While the same
task as that used in the present study (radial-arm maze in a novel room) does not
appear to have been used before to measure zif268 activity, it has been found that
performance of a spatial working memory task in the radial-arm maze can correlate
with hippocampal zif268 activity (Poirier et al., 2008a). Other studies also indicate
a role for zif268 in maintaining spatial information (e.g., Bozon et al., 2002). Thus,
it can be seen that the current evidence suggests that the changes in hippocampal
CREB phosphorylation following anterior thalamic damage disrupt several pathways,
including c-fos expression, that then impair spatial learning. While this description
does not preclude the additional involvement of other IEGs that are also regulated
by CREB, including zif268 (Guzowski, 2002), the lack of any thalamic lesion-induced
changes in hippocampal zif268 indicates that this IEG is principally under the control
of other sites.
This description, that anterior thalamic damage impairs spatial learning via its impact
on hippocampal function, including CREB/pCREB regulation, offers only one level of
analysis. This account does not explain functionally why, how, or when the anterior
thalamic nuclei might regulate hippocampal activity. A second issue is that the anterior
thalamic lesions in Experiment 2 sometimes produced a very restricted zone of cell
loss at the medial margin of the most rostral part of the septal hippocampus that
could potentially affect local hippocampal activity. Additional inter-hemispheric
comparisons for hippocampal CREB and pCREB were, therefore, conducted between the
hemisphere that was either intact (or had minimal cell loss) versus the hemisphere
with the greater cell loss (albeit very restricted). No differences were apparent
for hippocampal CREB or pCREB (both p > 0.1). It should also be added that all hippocampal
measurements were taken far away from any sites of possible pathology. An opposite
concern is that the anterior thalamic damage in Experiment 2 was typically more restricted
than that in Experiment 1 and the group sizes modest, so the findings may underestimate
the extent of distal dysfunction. While this may be true, the same thalamic lesions
were sufficient to severely impair T-maze alternation (Aggleton et al., 2009). A third
issue is that only one time period post surgery was examined, and so it cannot be
determined if the CREB/pCREB imbalance takes time to develop. It is, however, known
that anterior thalamic lesions impair spatial memory from the very first post-operative
session (Warburton et al., 1999) and that c-fos hypoactivity is present in the retrosplenial
cortex from the earliest time point so far measured, 1 week after surgery, to at least
1 year after surgery (Poirier and Aggleton, 2009).
The impact of the anterior thalamic lesions upon hippocampal CREB/pCREB balance could
be via the loss of direct connections between the two structures or via indirect connections,
such as those through the retrosplenial cortex. The latter proposal receives some
support from the marked IEG changes seen in the retrosplenial cortex for c-fos (Jenkins
et al., 2002a, 2004b; Poirier et al., 2008b; Poirier and Aggleton, 2009) and zif268
(Jenkins et al., 2004b; also present study). These combined IEG changes strongly predict
that the retrosplenial cortex should also show alterations in CREB/pCREB activity.
While counts involving all retrosplenial regions failed to support this prediction,
a significant reduction of pCREB was found when the analyses targeted just the granular
retrosplenial regions, i.e., those regions showing the most consistent IEG changes
following anterior thalamic damage (Jenkins et al., 2002a, 2004b; Poirier and Aggleton,
2009). The same retrosplenial subregions show a loss of neural plasticity, as demonstrated
by electrophysiological stimulation of retrosplenial tissue after anterior thalamic
lesions (Garden et al., 2009). Unlike the hippocampus, no interaction between retrosplenial
CREB and pCREB activity was found in the present study as levels of both markers tended
to decline, although this was not significant for CREB. This overall pattern of CREB
and pCREB results suggests that there could be indirect effects (via the retrosplenial
cortex) as well as direct effects, upon the hippocampus following anterior thalamic
damage.
Experiment 2 also looked at levels of the GAP-43 protein. GAP-43 is thought to regulate
axonal growth during development, including the targeting of other neurons for synapse
formation (Benowitz and Routtenberg, 1997; Wehrle et al., 2001). The same protein
is transiently upregulated after axotomy of CNS neurons and is, thereby, thought to
be involved in axonal regeneration after damage in the adult brain. A consequence
is that the sprouting of axotomized neurons is often associated with GAP-43 expression
in the developing and adult brain (Benowitz and Routtenberg, 1997; Wehrle et al.,
2001). Other evidence suggests that GAP-43 is involved in the signaling pathway that
provokes the decision for either axonal growth or cell death (retraction), i.e., opposite
cellular outcomes (Wehrle et al., 2001). For these reasons, particular interest focused
on the retrosplenial cortex, not only because this area receives dense direct inputs
from the anterior thalamic nuclei (van Groen and Wyss, 1990, 1992, 2003) but also
because anterior thalamic lesions reliably disrupt the expression of IEGs such as
c-fos and zif268 within the retrosplenial cortex (Jenkins et al., 2004b; Poirier and
Aggleton, 2009). Indeed, gene array studies show that the retrosplenial changes after
anterior thalamic lesions are pervasive, affecting a wide variety of other genes encoding
transcription factors, including brd8, fra-2, klf5, nfix, nr4a1, smad3, smarcc2 and
zfp9 (Poirier et al., 2008b). In contrast, cell numbers seem little affected in the
retrosplenial cortex after anterior thalamic damage (Jenkins et al., 2004b; Poirier
and Aggleton, 2009), raising the question of whether there might be more subtle white
matter changes. A complex array of changes could, therefore, have occurred in the
retrosplenial cortex in response to the thalamic surgeries, e.g., sprouting from within
the cortex (Carmichael et al., 2005) as well as the retraction of afferents. In fact,
the present study found no convincing evidence of changes in GAP-43 within the retrosplenial
cortex although this null result can only be regarded as preliminary until a range
of post-surgical intervals are examined (see also Steward, 1995).
In summary, the present study helps to highlight the special three-way relationship
between the anterior thalamic nuclei, the retrosplenial cortex, and the hippocampus
(Albasser et al., 2007; Vann et al., 2009). Not only are all three areas directly
interconnected but they are also thought to function together to support spatial learning
and memory (Sutherland and Rodriguez, 1989; Vann et al., 2009). This inter-relationship
appears to not only include the expression of IEGs such as c-fos and zif268, but also
how they are regulated by pCREB. Consequently, the present findings add additional
support to the concept of an ‘extended hippocampal system’ (Aggleton and Brown, 1999)
in which hippocampal function is dependent on distal sites such as the anterior thalamus
and retrosplenial cortex.